1,113
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Articles

Body Composition, Sarcopenic Obesity, and Cognitive Function in Older Adults: Findings From the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES) 1999–2002 and 2011–2014

, , &
Received 16 Mar 2023, Accepted 17 Mar 2024, Published online: 02 Apr 2024

Abstract

Objective

Sarcopenic-obesity (SO) is characterized by the concomitant presence of low muscle mass and high adiposity. This study explores the association of body composition and SO phenotypes with cognitive function in older adults.

Methods

Cross-sectional data in older adults (≥60 years) from NHANES 1999–2002 and 2011–2014 were used. In the 1999–2002 cohort, phenotypes were derived from body mass index (BMI) and dual-X-ray-absorptiometry, and cognition was assessed the by Digit-Symbol-Substitution-Test (DSST). In the 2011–2014 cohort, phenotypes were derived from BMI, waist-circumference (WC), and hand-grip-strength (HGS). Cognition was assessed using four tests: DSST, Animal Fluency, the Consortium-to-Establish-a-Registry-for-Alzheimer’s-Disease-Delayed-Recall, and Word Learning. Mediation analysis was conducted to evaluate the contribution of inflammation (C-reactive-protein, CRP) and insulin resistance (Homeostatic-Model-Assessment-for-Insulin-Resistance, HOMA-IR) to the association between body composition and cognitive outcomes.

Results

The SO phenotype had the lowest DSST mean scores (p < 0.05) and was associated with a significant risk of cognitive impairment [Odds Ratio (OR) = 1.9; 95%CI 1.0–3.7, p = 0.027] in the 1999–2002 cohort. A higher ratio of fat mass and fat free mass (FM/FFM) also showed a greater risk of cognitive impairment (OR = 2.0; 95%CI 1.3–3.1, p = 0.004). In the 2011–2014 cohort, the high WC-Low HGS group showed significantly lower scores on all four cognitive tests (p < 0.05) and a higher risk of cognitive impairment. CRP and HOMA-IR were significant partial mediators of the association between FM/FFM and DSST in the 1999–2002 cohort.

Conclusions

The SO phenotype was associated with a higher risk of cognitive impairment in older adults. Insulin resistance and inflammation may represent key mechanisms linking SO to the development of cognitive impairment.

Introduction

Obesity is linked to metabolic and vascular dysregulation (i.e., insulin resistance, inflammation, loss of endothelial integrity), which may contribute to the impairment of brain health and increase the risk of dementia (Citation1). However, the strength and direction of the associations between adiposity and brain health appear to be influenced by factors, such as aging, gender, and body fat distribution (i.e., central vs peripheral). For example, middle-age obesity is a potential risk factor for later-life dementia (Citation2, Citation3) and adiposity appears to have a health protective role in very old individuals (>80 years old), a phenomenon that has been termed the “obesity paradox.” A cross-sectional study indicated that a higher body mass index (BMI ≥25 kg/m2) and visceral adiposity [measured by computerized tomography (CT)] were associated with lower cognitive performance in individuals <70 years, but not in those ≥70 years (Citation4). The association between BMI and dementia risk was inverted in 2,798 participants from the Cardiovascular Health Study (CHS) Cognition Study as overweight status was not associated with dementia risk whereas obesity (BMI > 30 kg/m2) was associated with a reduced risk of dementia (Hazard Ratio (HR), 0.63; 95% Confidence Interval (CI), 0.44–0.91) compared to a normal BMI (20–25 kg/m2) (Citation5). However, adiposity indices, such as BMI or waist circumference (WC) do not provide information on individual body components (i.e., mass, distribution); hence, the assessment of body composition may provide greater accuracy in the prediction of cognitive impairment and dementia risk (Citation6).

A significant reduction in skeletal muscle mass (SMM) and function is defined as sarcopenia (Citation7), which has been linked to cognitive impairment and dementia (Citation8, Citation9). Bae et al (Citation10) found in 840 older Japanese individuals (≥65 years) that a lower fat free mass (FFM) and appendicular muscle mass were associated with an increased risk of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) in men and a higher lean body mass (LM), which did not include bone mineral mass, was associated with reduced risk of cognitive impairment in older adults (Citation11, Citation12).

The co-existence in the same individual of reduced muscle mass and/or strength with an increased adiposity characterizes the phenotype of sarcopenic obesity (SO) (Citation13), which may be associated with a greater risk for adverse health outcomes compared to either sarcopenia or obesity independently (Citation13, Citation14). The concomitant occurrence of obesity and sarcopenia could synergistically amplify their independent effects on inflammation, insulin resistance, and vascular dysfunction which, in turn, could promote additional losses of muscle mass (Citation15) and predispose to further weight gain. Several studies have investigated the association between SO and cognitive function with contrasting results, which could be explained by differences in body composition methods, diagnostic definitions of SO, study design, and phenotypic characteristics of the populations (Citation16–22).

This study explores the association between body composition and cognitive function in older adults (≥60 years) from the NHANES 1999–2002 and 2011–2014 cohorts. The aim was to explore and compare the independent associations of two different approaches for the assessment of SO with cognition and whether the SO the phenotype might confer a greater risk for cognitive impairment in both cohorts. In the 1999–2002 cohort, the association between dual X-ray absorptiometry (DXA)-based measurements of lean body mass and adiposity with measures of executive function, assessed by the Digit-Symbol-Substitution-Test (DSST) scores, was explored. Age, gender, and BMI specific DXA-based body composition models of SO (Citation23–25) were applied and the association with DSST scores was investigated. In the 2011–2014 cohort, adiposity was assessed by anthropometry (BMI, WC), and hand-grip strength (HGS) was used as a functional measure of muscle mass. The independent and interactive effects of adiposity and HGS on domains of cognitive function (i.e., memory, attention, executive) were evaluated. A mediation analysis was conducted to evaluate the influence of inflammation (C-reactive-Protein: CRP) and insulin resistance (Homeostatic Model Assessment for Insulin Resistance: HOMA-IR) on the associations between SO and cognitive function.

Methods

Data and study population

Cross-sectional data from the 1999–2002 and 2011–2014 waves of the NHANES programme were used to examine the association between body composition and cognitive function in individuals aged 60–85 years. All DXA datasets are released by NHANES on the CDC website (Citation26). Ethical approval was obtained from the NHANES Institutional Review Board and the NCHS Research Ethics Review Board. The protocol number of NHANES 1999–2002 is Protocol #98-12 and the number of NHANES 2011–2014 is Protocol #2011-17 (Citation27). Individuals with incomplete cognitive test and body composition (e.g., DXA and/or anthropometric) data were excluded.

Anthropometry

NHANES (1999–2002)

Body measurements were recorded for all participants by a trained examiner in the mobile examination center. In situations where participants had to leave the mobile examination center early and were unable to complete the body measurement component, at a minimum, weight and standing height or recumbent length were measured. Body weight, height, and BMI were included in the dataset. Height (cm) was measured using a stadiometer. Weight (kg) was measured using an electronic digital scale calibrated in kilograms (kg). BMI was calculated as weight (kg) divided by height in meters (m) squared.

Body composition assessment was undertaken by DXA (Hologic QDR 4500A). Participants were not eligible for a DXA scan if they were pregnant, weighed more than 136 kg, or if they were taller than 1.96 m. In addition, participants were not eligible if they had been exposed to radiographic contrast material in the past 7 days or nuclear medicine in the past 3 days. Only complete DXA data of eligible participants were included. The DXA scans provided bone and soft tissue measurements for the total body, arms and legs, trunk, and head. Body composition variables derived from DXA measurements included FM, Lean Body Mass (LBM), and Bone Mineral Mass (BMM). Fat free mass (FFM) was calculated as the sum of LBM and BMM. Appendicular Skeletal Mass (ASM) was calculated as the sum of LBM in arms and legs. FM, FFM, and ASM indexes were calculated by dividing each variable by height (m) squared (Citation24, Citation25).

NHANES (2011–2014)

Body measurements were collected by trained health technicians in the mobile examination center. Body weight (kg) and height (cm) were measured, and BMI was calculated. Waist circumference (cm) was measured just above the uppermost lateral border of the right ilium.

In both cohorts, participants were stratified by BMI and WC according to recommended cut-offs. BMI categories included underweight (<18.5 kg/m2), normal weight (≥18.5 and <25 kg/m2), overweight (≥25 and <30 kg/m2) and obesity (≥30 kg/m2) (Citation28). For men, a waist circumference (WC) below 102 cm was defined as “normal or moderately high WC” and ≥102 cm was “high WC.” For women, below 88 cm was “normal or moderately high WC,” and more than or equal to 88 cm was “high WC” (Citation29).

Hand-grip strength (HGS)

NHANES (2011–2014)

Muscle strength was assessed from the isometric grip strength using a handgrip dynamometer (Takei Hand Grip Dynamometer, Japan). Participants were asked to remove hand and wrist jewelry. The grip size of the dynamometer was also adjusted to ensure optimal performance and participants completed two warm-up exercises. Participants were then asked to squeeze hand-grip dynamometer as hard as possible for three times on each hand (alternating hands between trials with a 60 s rest between measurements on the same hand). The average of the combined grip strength (the sum of the largest reading from each hand) was calculated and used in the analysis and stated in kg. This variable was not calculated for participants who only performed the test on one hand. HGS cut-offs for men and women to identify individuals at-risk of sarcopenia in older adults were <30 and 20 kg, respectively (Citation30).

Assessment of body composition phenotypes

NHANES (1999–2002)

Baumgartner model (Citation23, Citation31)

The model was derived using DXA data from the New Mexico Elder Health Survey, 1993–1995. Sarcopenia was identified if appendicular skeletal muscle mass index (ASMI) was lower than 7.26 kg/m2 in men and 5.45 kg/m2 in women. Obesity was defined if FM% was >27% in men and 38% in women. The cut-offs for ASMI corresponded to the lower 2SD value derived from the distribution of ASMI in a healthy young normal weight population. The following body composition phenotypes can be derived: (1) sarcopenic (low ASMI and low FM%), normal (high ASMI and low FM%), obese (high ASMI and high FM%), and SO (low ASMI and high FM%).

Prado-Siervo model (Citation24)

This model used DXA body composition data to operationalize the large variability in body-composition phenotypes by taking into account the individual effects of age, gender, and body mass on body components. Age, gender, and BMI-specific reference curves were derived for each body composition variable using the DXA data from the 1999–2004 NHANES survey. The Prado-Siervo model includes the following four phenotypes: LA-HM (low adiposity-high muscle mass), HA-HM (high adiposity-high muscle mass), LA-LM (low adiposity-low muscle mass), and HA-LM (high adiposity-low muscle mass). The cutoffs applied for each phenotype are LA-HM (FMI: 0–49.99; ASMI: 50–100); HA-HM (FMI: 50–100; ASMI: 50–100); LA-LM (FMI: 0–49.99; ASMI: 0–49.99), and HA-LM (FMI: 50–100; ASMI: 0–49.99).

Siervo-Prado model (Citation25)

This model used DXA body composition data to calculate the ratio between FM and FFM (FM:FFM) and between trunk fat mass (TrFM) and ASM (TrFM:ASM). Age, gender, and BMI-specific reference curves were developed for both ratios. Individuals were classified as low (<15th centile), normal (15–84.99th centile), or high (≥85th centile), with the higher category indicating the SO phenotype.

NHANES (2011–2014)

Age (≥60 years old) and gender specific cutoffs for HGS and gender-specific cut-offs for WC were used to derive body composition phenotypes which include (1) low HGS–high WC (SO phenotype), (2) high HGS–high WC, (3) low HGS–low WC, and (4) high HGS–low WC (reference group). The ratio between WC and HGS (WC-HGS-R) was also used as a simple method to classify SO (high WC to low HGS). The WC-HGS-R was analyzed as a continuous variable or divided into tertiles with the highest tertile indicating the SO phenotype and the lowest tertile used in the analysis as a reference group.

Cognitive assessment

NHANES (1999–2002)

In the 1999–2002 cohort, cognition was assessed using the DSST. In this test, participants copied symbols that were paired with numbers. Using the key provided at the top of the exercise form, the participant drew the symbol under the corresponding number. Sample items were provided for initial practice. Participants who were unable to complete any of the sample items did not continue to the definitive test. The score was calculated as the number of correct symbols drawn within 120 s. One point was given for each correctly drawn symbol completed within the time limit. The maximum score was 133 with higher scores indicating greater cognitive function. The DSST did not assume that participants who had a physical or mental impairment would be unable to do the sample, and there was no medical condition data of participants about dementia or Alzheimer’s Disease (AD) in NHANES 1999–2002 (only relevant stroke). Thus, this analysis included adults aged 60 years and older with all cognitive states (i.e., with and without dementia). DSST provides information on multiple cognitive domains including response speed (motor skills), sustained attention, visual spatial skills, associative learning, and memory. DSST scores were entered in the analyses as a continuous variable. A DSST cut-off of <40 was applied to identify subjects with impaired cognitive function (Citation32).

NHANES (2011–2014)

In 2011–2014, a series of assessments were re-introduced, including (1) word learning and recall modules from the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD); (2) the Animal Fluency (AF) test; and (3) the DSST. Cognitive outcomes were entered into the analyses as continuous variables. Cognitive outcomes were run as separate models including AF, DSST, CERAD Word Learning subtest (CERAD-WL), and CERAD Delayed Recall test (CERAD-DR) (Citation33). The AF test examined categorical verbal fluency, a component of executive function. Scores have been shown to discriminate between persons with normal cognitive functioning compared with those with MCI and more severe forms of cognitive impairment, such as AD. CERAD-WL assessed immediate and delayed learning ability for new verbal information (memory sub-domain). Following CERAD-WL trials, CERAD-DR was administered 8–10 min after the completion of both other cognitive exercises (AF and DSST). However, cognitive outcomes were also stratified by specific cut-offs for each cognitive outcome to classify individuals as cognitively impaired and non-impaired. The cognitive scores were stratified by cut-offs and lower scores were considered as impaired, depending on the type of cognitive test. A brief description of the cognitive scores and calculations is provided in the . Cognitive impairment was defined by applying the following cut-offs to each cognitive test: AF <14, DSST <40, CERAD-WL <17, and CERAD-DR <5 (Citation32, Citation34).

Table 1. Characteristics of study participants included in the analyses of the 1999–2002 and 2011–2014 datasets.a

Covariates: socio-demographic, lifestyle, and laboratory biomarkers

Covariates included: age, gender, education, marital status, annual household income, number of morbidities (diabetes, kidney problems, anemia, arthritis, congestive heart failure, coronary heart disease, cancer or malignancy), stroke, blood pressure, physical activity, medications related cognitive interactions, smoking, depression, alcohol use, energy consumption, CRP (only for NHANES 1999–2002), and metabolic markers (fasting glucose and insulin, both cohorts). Fasting glucose and insulin concentrations were used to calculate the Homeostatic Model of Assessment (HOMA-IR) of insulin resistance (Citation12). In NHANES 1999–2002, CRP was quantified by latex-enhanced nephelometry, plasma glucose was calculated by an enzymatic hexokinase (HK) method and plasma insulin was assessed by radioimmunoassay (RIA). In NHANES 2011–2014, plasma glucose was calculated by the HK method and plasma insulin was assessed by a Roche chemiluminescent immunoassay performed on the Elecsys 2010 analyzer.

Statistical analysis

All data were analyzed using the SPSS complex sample module (Citation35) version 28.0 (IBM Corp, Armonk, NY, USA). p-Value <0.05 was considered as statistically significant. Complex-survey analysis was applied to account for the sampling strategy of the NHANES survey and conducted according to NHANES approved protocols. Analysis of the NHANES datasets followed the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) guidelines as multiple survey cycles were combined and specific population-weights were applied according to the suggested weighting methodology (Citation36). The results were presented as means ± standard deviation or frequency (%) according to the characteristics of the variables. Complex Samples General Linear Model (CSGLM) regression analysis was used to analyze the association between body composition variables and cognitive variables. Complex Samples Logistic (CSL) regression analysis was performed to evaluate the association of body composition with odds of cognitive impairment. The distribution of residuals was checked to ensure the validity of the regression models. Analyses were adjusted for potential confounding variables which were available in two consecutive cycles of the NHANES (1999–2002 and 2011–2014). Analyses were conducted in both cohorts after stratification by age (60–70 and >70 years).

Mediation analysis was conducted in both analyses to quantify the extent to which biological variables (CRP and HOMA-IR for NHANES 1999–2002, HOMA-IR for NHANES 2011–2014) influence the association between body composition and cognitive function by using the SPSS Macro developed by Preacher and Hayes (Citation37, Citation38). The mediation analysis model is described in . A full list of the covariates and mediators entered in each analysis is provided in the .

Figure 1. Mediation model for the association between sarcopenic obesity and cognitive impairment and sarcopenic obesity with insulin resistance (IR) or inflammation as mediators (Models 1 and 2). The direct effect was identified as c path, indirect effect by the b path, total effect (C′) was the combination of direct and indirect effects. The direct effect measures the extent to which the dependent variable changes when the independent variable increases by one unit and the mediator variable remains unaltered. In contrast, the indirect effect measures the extent to which the dependent variable changes when the independent variable is held constant and the mediator variable changes by the amount it would have changed had the independent variable increased by one unit. The indirect effect constitutes the extent to which the X variable influences the Y variable through the mediator. In linear systems, the total effect is equal to the sum of the direct and indirect (C′ + AB in the model above). A positive sign indicates the same direction (complimentary mediator) of the association between the exposure and outcome whereas a negative sign was considered having an opposite effect (competitive mediator).

Figure 1. Mediation model for the association between sarcopenic obesity and cognitive impairment and sarcopenic obesity with insulin resistance (IR) or inflammation as mediators (Models 1 and 2). The direct effect was identified as c path, indirect effect by the b path, total effect (C′) was the combination of direct and indirect effects. The direct effect measures the extent to which the dependent variable changes when the independent variable increases by one unit and the mediator variable remains unaltered. In contrast, the indirect effect measures the extent to which the dependent variable changes when the independent variable is held constant and the mediator variable changes by the amount it would have changed had the independent variable increased by one unit. The indirect effect constitutes the extent to which the X variable influences the Y variable through the mediator. In linear systems, the total effect is equal to the sum of the direct and indirect (C′ + AB in the model above). A positive sign indicates the same direction (complimentary mediator) of the association between the exposure and outcome whereas a negative sign was considered having an opposite effect (competitive mediator).

Table 2. Complex samples general linear model (CSGLM) to test the association between the classifications of body composition (factor) and cognitive function (dependent variable) before and after adjusted model 1999–2002.Table Footnote*

Results

The flowcharts describing the selection of participants included in the final analysis for the NHANES 1999–2002 (Figure S1) and NHANES 2011–2014 (Figure S2) cohorts are reported in the OSM. The body composition characteristics of the population stratified according to the models of body composition phenotypes are shown in Table S3 of the OSM.

NHANES 1999–2002: The final dataset consisted of 2,544 participants with 43.3% as men. Participants mean age (±SE) was 70.44 ± 0.27 years, and 70.1% had a BMI ≥ 25 kg/m2. 83.8% of participants were non-Hispanic white and ∼70% of the participants completed high school or above. Descriptive characteristics of the sample are provided in . The prevalence of the phenotypes derived from the body composition models is shown in . There was a difference between the Baumgartner and the Prado-Siervo models in estimating the prevalence of the SO phenotype in the cohort (14.5 vs 21.9%, respectively).

Figure 2. The prevalence of body composition phenotype in the NHANES 1999–2002 and the NHANES 2011–2014. FM = fat mass, FFM = fat free mass, TrFM = truncal fat mass, ASM = appendicular skeletal mass, WC = waist circumference, HGS = hand grip strength.

Figure 2. The prevalence of body composition phenotype in the NHANES 1999–2002 and the NHANES 2011–2014. FM = fat mass, FFM = fat free mass, TrFM = truncal fat mass, ASM = appendicular skeletal mass, WC = waist circumference, HGS = hand grip strength.

In adjusted models, FM (kg), FM (%), FMI (kg/m2), and FM/FFM ratio were significantly associated with the DSST score (Beta Coefficient ± SE = 0.1 ± 0.1, 0.2 ± 0.1, 0.3 ± 0.1, and 8.6 ± 2.8, respectively; all p < 0.05) (Table S4 of the OSM). DSST scores differed between the body composition phenotypes based on Baumgartner’s model (p = 0.022) with the HA-LM phenotype having significantly lower scores than the HA-HM phenotype (p < 0.05). No significant differences between phenotypes were identified for the Prado-Siervo and the two ratios (FM/FFM, TrFM/ASM) models (). The SO phenotype identified by Baumgartner’s model had a significant risk of cognitive impairment compared to the reference group (HA-LM vs LA-HM, OR = 1.9; CI 95% 1.0–3.7, p = 0.027). In addition, the highest FM/FFM centile (≥85th) also showed a greater risk of cognitive impairment (OR = 2.0; CI 95% 1.3–3.1, p = 0.004) compared to the normal centile group (15–84.9th) (). The HA-LM phenotype for both Baumgartner and Prado-Siervo models was significantly associated with cognitive impairment in subjects younger than 70 years old but associations were not significant in subjects older than 70 years (Table S5 of the OSM).

Table 3. Complex samples logistic (CSL) regression analysis of the association between the classifications of body composition (factor) and cognitive function (dependent variable) before and after adjusted model 1999–2002.*

NHANES 2011–2014: The final dataset included 3,395 participants (men = 45.1%). Participant mean age (±SE) was 69.5 ± 0.1 years and 73.1% of participants had a BMI ≥ 25 kg/m2. Descriptive characteristics of the sample are provided in .

A higher WC was characterized by significantly higher CERAD-WL scores compared to lower WC (score 20.0 and 19.5, respectively; p = 0.019). The LWC-LHGS and HWC-LHGS were also characterized by significantly lower scores for the AF, DSST, CERAD-WL, and CERAD-DR (p < 0.001) cognitive tests. The highest tertile of WC-HGS-R index had significantly lower AF and DSST scores (p < 0.001 and p < 0.001, respectively) ().

Table 4. Complex samples general linear model (CSGLM) regression analysis of the association between the classifications of body composition and grip strength (factor) and cognitive function (dependent variable) before and after adjusted model 2011–2014.*

A higher WC (≥102 cm in men and ≥88 cm in women) had a lower risk of cognitive impairment (CERAD-WL, OR = 0.6; CI 95%: 0.5–0.8; p < 0.001) compared to low WC. However, the LWC-LHGS (AF, OR = 2.2; CI 95%: 1.2–4.0; DSST, OR = 2.9; CI 95%: 1.8–4.9; CERAD-DR, OR = 2.1; CI 95%: 1.2–3.4; all p < 0.05) and the HWC–LHGS (AF, OR = 2.2; CI 95%: 1.4–3.5; DSST, OR = 2.5; CI 95%: 1.6–4.0; CERAD-WL, OR = 1.6; CI 95%: 1.1–2.4; CERAD-DR, OR = 1.6; CI 95%: 1.1–2.5; all p < 0.05) phenotypes had a higher risk of cognitive impairment compared to LWC–HHGS. The highest WC-HGS-R tertile had a greater risk of cognitive impairment (AF, OR = 1.6; CI 95%: 1.1–2.1; DSST, OR = 1.4; CI 95%: 1.0–1.9; all p < 0.05) compared to lowest tertile 1 (). The LWC–LHGS phenotype was significantly associated with a greater risk of cognitive impairment assessed by the DSST and CERAD-WL and DR in subjects younger than 70 years old (Table S6 of the OSM).

Table 5. Complex samples logistic (CSL) regression analysis of the association between the classifications of body composition and grip strength (factor) and cognitive function (dependent variable) before and after adjusted model 2011–2014.*

Mediation analysis

NHANES 1999–2002

The mediation analysis showed a significant indirect effect of both CRP (FM/FFM, b = −0.5, TrFM/ASM, b = −0.3) and HOMA-IR (FM/FFM, b = −1.4, TrFM/ASM, b = −1.9) in mediating the impact of FM/FFM and TrFM/ASM on DSST. Furthermore, the direct effect of FM/FFM and TrFM/ASM on DSST was also significant; hence, CRP or HOMA-IR may partially mediate the association between FM/FFM and TrFM/ASM and DSST (). The mediation model 2 () assessed the concomitant role of both CRP and HOMA-IR on the association between body composition variables and cognitive function (DSST) in the NHANES 1999–2002 cohort. The analysis showed a significant indirect effect on FM/FFM and TrFM/ASM on DSST, but this was only mediated by HOMA-IR (FM/FFM, b = −1.4, TrFM/ASM, b = −1.9). A direct effect of FM/FFM (b = 12.2, p < 0.005) and TrFM/ASM (b = 8.9, p < 0.05) on DSST was also found; hence, a partial mediation of the association between FM/FFM and TrFM/ASM and DSST was found for HOMA-IR ().

Table 6. Mediation analysis of CRP and HOMA-IR on the association between body composition variables and cognitive function (NHANES 1999–2002).

NHANES 2011–2014

The study assessed the mediating role of HOMA-IR on the association between body composition variables (WC-HGS-R) and cognitive function in NHANES 2011–2014. The results show there were no significant indirect effects of the impact of HOMA-IR on cognitive performance (AF, DSST, CERAD-WL, and CERAD-DR). However, the direct effect of body composition (WC-HGS-R) on cognitive function was significant (AF, DSST, CERAD-WL, and CERAD-DR). Therefore, HOMA-IR was a mediator of the association between body WC-HGS-R and cognitive function in the NHANES 2011–2014 cohort ().

Discussion

The results showed that BMI, WC, and other individual body composition parameters (FM, FFM, ASM) were overall poor predictors of cognitive impairment. The SO phenotype, based on the concomitant measurement of FM and muscle mass and/or muscle function, was more closely associated with cognitive functions in both NHANES cohorts. The mediation analysis showed that insulin resistance, assessed by HOMA-IR, may explain the associations between the SO phenotype and cognitive function.

Older adults (≥60 years) with obesity (BMI ≥30 kg/m2) tended to have higher cognitive scores than other BMI groups; in addition, BMI was not significantly associated with an increased risk of cognitive impairment in independent analyses conducted in the 1999–2002 and 2011–2014 NHANES cohorts. Hou et al. (Citation39) showed that a BMI between 24.0 and 27.9 kg/m2 was significantly related to a decreased risk of cognitive impairment (OR = 0.5, 95% CI = 0.3–0.7, p < 0.001) in 1,100 Chinese subjects aged 60–98 years; however, a significant association was found between abdominal obesity, measured by WC, and increased risk of cognitive impairment. The predictive role of BMI for cognitive and dementia risk has been challenged in other studies as the statistical significance of the associations may vary with age (Citation5). Middle-aged obesity has been associated with increased dementia risk in later life (older than 65 years) (Citation2) but having a BMI > 30 kg/m2 in older age does not necessarily predict dementia risk (Citation5).

Measures of central adiposity, such as WC or truncal FM, have been considered as better adiposity risk factors due to the closer role played by central adiposity in the pathogenesis of cardiovascular and metabolic diseases (Citation40). However, the association of WC with risk for cognitive impairment has been inconsistent across studies (Citation41, Citation42), which was also reported in the analysis conducted in the NHANES 2011–2014 cohort as we found an inverse association between WC and CERAD-WL.

High FM, measured by different body composition methods (i.e., DXA, bioelectrical impedance), has been inconsistently associated with the risk of cognitive impairment (Citation43–48). In a cross-sectional analysis using data from the Canadian Longitudinal Study on Aging (N = 30,097), a higher total FM measured by DXA was associated with lower cognitive performance on the AF, Stroop interference, and reaction time tasks (Citation43). In another cross-sectional analysis of more than 9,000 middle-aged and older participants, DSST cognitive scores were lower with increasing FM percentage and visceral adipose tissue measured by bioelectrical impedance analysis (BIA) and magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), respectively (Citation44). An analysis conducted on more than 20,000 individuals in the UK Biobank cohort showed that FM% measured by BIA was linked to lower cortical thickness, gray matter, and subcortical structure volumes and to lower working memory (Citation45). Several studies have also observed that a higher FM was associated with a lower risk of cognitive impairment in older individuals (>70 years) (Citation46–48), suggesting the existence of an age-interaction for the association of adiposity with cognitive function and dementia risk (Citation49). Low FFM or other similar functional or quantitative measurements of muscle mass (i.e., lean body mass, ASM, HGS) have been more frequently associated with a higher risk of cognitive impairment in different age groups (Citation50–54). A prospective analysis conducted in the Canadian Longitudinal Study on Aging showed that the presence of low appendicular lean soft tissue mass at baseline was associated with faster 3-year cognitive decline in executive functions and psychomotor speed (Citation50). However, several studies have shown that functional measures of muscle mass (i.e., HGS, gait speed) may be better predictors of cognitive function compared to quantitative measures (Citation51–53).

The SO phenotype attempts to amalgamate the physiological effects of both high FM and low muscle mass into a single diagnostic definition, which may provide a greater predictive sensitivity to estimate risk for impaired cardio-metabolic and brain health compared to FM or sarcopenia alone (Citation13). This analysis showed that DXA-based models of the SO phenotype defined by different definitions [HA-LM (Baumgartner), HA-LM (Prado-Siervo), FM/FFM centile ≥85th, TrFM/ASM centile ≥85th] tended to have lower DSST scores and were associated with higher risk of cognitive impairment in the NHANES 1999–2002 cohort. Due to the lack of quantitative measures of body composition (i.e., DXA or BIA) in the 2011–2014 cohort, a different approach was adopted to operationalize the definition of SO. This was based on WC data as a measure of central obesity and HGS data were used as functional parameters of sarcopenia, allowing to test the predictive value of these two user-friendly and affordable measurements. In fact, the LWC-LHGS and HWC-LHGS phenotypes were significantly associated with a higher risk of cognitive impairment, which may suggest a greater predictive role of low muscle function. The results also revealed that the highest tertile for the FM: muscle strength ratio (higher FM and lower muscle strength) was associated with a greater risk of cognitive impairment, suggesting that the WC-HGS Ratio may represent a better risk predictor compared to the individual measurement of WC or HGS. A previous study used a similar approach by evaluating the association of the handgrip/bodyweight ratio (HGS/BW) with the risk of metabolic syndrome in 5,026 participants (mean age 51.2 years) and found that participants in the lowest tertile of HGS/BW had a higher risk of metabolic impairment (Citation55). To our knowledge, no study has so far evaluated the association of the WC-HGS Ratio with cognitive function. However, the association of SO with cognitive function has been explored in other studies, which overall found a superior predictive value of the SO phenotype for risk of cognitive impairment compared to obesity or sarcopenia alone (Citation16, Citation17, Citation19–22, Citation53, Citation56, Citation57). However, studies showed large differences in the definitions of SO, population characteristics, study design and methods of body composition, and cognitive function assessment, which may explain some of the differences between studies regarding the associations with various measures of cognitive domains. Five studies (Citation16, Citation21, Citation22, Citation56, Citation57) used BIA to assess body composition followed by DXA which was used in three studies (Citation19, Citation20, Citation53). Three studies (Citation16, Citation19, Citation53) used HGS as a functional measure of sarcopenia which was combined in two studies (Citation16, Citation53) with measurements of FFM obtained from BIA or DXA. The only prospective analysis used data from 5822 older participants recruited as part of the National Health and Aging Trends study. Baseline data on HGS and BMI and prospective data on cognitive function collected over an 8-year follow-up were included in the analysis; the study found that the risk of impaired cognitive function was not significant in obesity alone (HR 0.98; 95% CI 0.82–1.16), but was significantly higher in sarcopenia (low HGS, HR 1.60; 95% CI 1.42–1.80) and SO (low HGS and high BMI, HR 1.20; 95% CI 1.03–1.40) (Citation17). Another paper used data from the NHANES 1999–2002 cohort (1127 older participants) to test the association between DSST scores and SO, which was based on waist circumference and DXA measurements of muscle mass (Citation19). The analysis was stratified by age (60–69 and ≥70 years) and found that the SO was associated with lower DSST scores only in adults aged 70 years and over, which was not found in our study. The study also found an inverse relationship between HOMA-IT with DSST scores which accounted for ∼20% of the association between SO and cognitive function (Citation19). Three studies found a significant association between ASM/FM (Citation22) (lowest tertile) and FM/FFM (Citation56, Citation57) (highest tertile) ratios with impaired global (MoCA) or domain-specific (i.e., attention, language, visuospatial abilities, and immediate and delayed memory recall) cognitive functions.

The mediation analysis showed that CRP and HOMA-IR may account partially for the association between FM/FFM and TrFM/ASM ratios and DSST scores. However, if both CRP and HOMA-IR were entered in the mediation analysis, HOMA-IR was the only competitive mediator of the association between SO ratios and cognitive function. The role of inflammation in the pathogenesis of insulin resistance is known (Citation58), which may suggest that insulin resistance could represent the “effector” mechanism linking SO to the deterioration of cognitive functions. Age-related cognitive impairment was independently associated with HOMA-IR (Citation59) and was a significant mediator of the association of SO with DSST scores for subjects aged 70 years and over (Citation19).

The study has several strengths including a large sample size representative of the US population, a cross-comparison of body composition methods and approaches to define SO, and validated cognitive tests. This study minimized the effects of potential confounding variables (i.e., demographics, comorbidity, blood pressure, physical activity, medications related cognitive interactions, smoking, depression, alcohol use, and energy consumption) by including them in the models as covariates to obtain more accurate estimates. There are also some limitations. The cross-sectional design does not allow the ascertainment of the causality of the associations between body composition phenotypes and cognitive outcomes. Depression was included as a covariate in the analyses; however, in the 1999–2002 cohort, patients with depression were identified only if they were taking antidepressant medications, leading to a possible underestimation of depression prevalence. Conversely, in the 2011–2014 cohort depression was identified by using a validated depression screening questionnaire (Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ)-9) (Citation60, Citation61). A single cognitive test was used in the 1999–2002 cohort, which may allow for a thorough assessment of multiple cognitive domains. However, the DSST is a sensitive measure of various cognitive domains and DSST scores correlate with the diagnosis of cognitive dysfunction and changes in cognitive function across a wide range of clinical populations (Citation62). Some of the significant differences in cognition between body composition phenotypes were small and the biological and clinical relevance of these significant results needs to be interpreted with caution.

Conclusions

This is the first study to test in two independent cohorts of older individuals the association of body composition phenotypes and different definitions of SO with global and domain-specific measures of cognitive function. Overall, to the best of our knowledge, SO was associated with a greater risk of cognitive impairment in both cohorts but SO definitions differed in their associations with cognitive impairment. This further emphasizes the need for a standardized diagnostic approach for the identification of SO cases. Insulin resistance may represent a key mechanism linking SO to the development of cognitive impairment, and possibly, dementia onset.

Author contributions

MS is the guarantor of this work and, as such, had full access to all the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. MS and UB conceived and designed the study. UB and MS conducted the analysis and wrote the manuscript. All authors contributed to the analysis, discussion, and interpretation of data, and reviewed/critically edited the manuscript. All authors have read and approved the final manuscript.

Supplemental material

Supplemental Material

Download MS Word (67.6 KB)

Disclosure statement

I.A.M. was a member of the UK Government Scientific Advisory Committee on Nutrition, the Mars Scientific Advisory Council, the Mars Europe Nutrition Advisory Board, the Nestle Research Scientific Advisory Board, the Novozymes Scientific Advisory Board, and was a Scientific Adviser to the Waltham Center for Pet Nutrition until 2020. On August 1, 2020, he became Professor Emeritus at the University of Nottingham and took up the post of Scientific Director of the Nestle Institute of Health Sciences in Lausanne, Switzerland, which terminated in August 2022. Other authors: no conflicts to declare.

Data availability statement

The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request. The NHANES data are publicly available at https://wwwn.cdc.gov/Nchs/Nhanes.

Additional information

Funding

None to declare.

References

  • Gustafson DR. Adiposity and cognitive decline: underlying mechanisms. JAD. 2012;30(s2):S97–S112. doi:10.3233/JAD-2012-120487.
  • Whitmer RA, Gunderson EP, Barrett-Connor E, Quesenberry CPJr., Yaffe K. Obesity in middle age and future risk of dementia: a 27 year longitudinal population based study. BMJ. 2005;330(7504):1360. doi:10.1136/bmj.38446.466238.E0.
  • Singh-Manoux A, Dugravot A, Shipley M, Brunner EJ, Elbaz A, Sabia S, Kivimaki M. Obesity trajectories and risk of dementia: 28 years of follow-up in the Whitehall II study. Alzheimers Dement. 2018;14(2):178–86. doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2017.06.2637.
  • Yoon DH, Choi SH, Yu JH, Ha JH, Ryu SH, Park DH. The relationship between visceral adiposity and cognitive performance in older adults. Age Ageing. 2012;41(4):456–61. doi:10.1093/ageing/afs018.
  • Fitzpatrick AL, Kuller LH, Lopez OL, Diehr P, O’Meara ES, Longstreth WTJr., Luchsinger JA. Midlife and late-life obesity and the risk of dementia: cardiovascular health study. Arch Neurol. 2009;66(3):336–42. doi:10.1001/archneurol.2008.582.
  • Kanaya AM, Lindquist K, Harris TB, Launer L, Rosano C, Satterfield S, Yaffe K, Health ABC Study. Total and regional adiposity and cognitive change in older adults: the health, aging and body composition (abc) study. Arch Neurol. 2009;66(3):329–35. doi:10.1001/archneurol.2008.570.
  • Santilli V, Bernetti A, Mangone M, Paoloni M. Clinical definition of sarcopenia. Clin Cases Miner Bone Metab. 2014;11(3):177. doi:10.11138/ccmbm/2014.11.3.177.
  • Chang K-V, Hsu T-H, Wu W-T, Huang K-C, Han D-S. Association between sarcopenia and cognitive impairment: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Am Med Dir Assoc. 2016;17(12):1164.e7–15. doi:10.1016/j.jamda.2016.09.013.
  • Nishiguchi S, Yamada M, Shirooka H, Nozaki Y, Fukutani N, Tashiro Y, Hirata H, Yamaguchi M, Tasaka S, Matsushita T, et al. Sarcopenia as a risk factor for cognitive deterioration in community-dwelling older adults: a 1-year prospective study. J Am Med Dir Assoc. 2016;17(4):372.e375–8. doi:10.1016/j.jamda.2015.12.096.
  • Bae S, Shimada H, Park H, Lee S, Makizako H, Doi T, Yoshida D, Tsutsumimoto K, Anan Y, Suzuki T. Association between body composition parameters and risk of mild cognitive impairment in older Japanese adults. Geriatr Gerontol Int. 2017;17(11):2053–9. doi:10.1111/ggi.13018.
  • Cui C, Mackey RH, Shaaban CE, Kuller LH, Lopez OL, Sekikawa A. Associations of body composition with incident dementia in older adults: cardiovascular health study-cognition study. Alzheimers Dement. 2020;16(10):1402–11. doi:10.1002/alz.12125.
  • Noh H-M, Oh S, Song HJ, Lee EY, Jeong J-Y, Ryu O-H, Hong K-S, Kim D-H. Relationships between cognitive function and body composition among community-dwelling older adults: a cross-sectional study. BMC Geriatr. 2017;17(1):259. doi:10.1186/s12877-017-0651-9.
  • Donini LM, Busetto L, Bischoff SC, Cederholm T, Ballesteros-Pomar MD, Batsis JA, Bauer JM, Boirie Y, Cruz-Jentoft AJ, Dicker D, et al. Definition and diagnostic criteria for sarcopenic obesity: ESPEN and EASO consensus statement. Obes Facts. 2022;15(3):321–35. doi:10.1159/000521241.
  • Roh E, Choi KM. Health consequences of sarcopenic obesity: a narrative review. Front Endocrinol. 2020;11:332. doi:10.3389/fendo.2020.00332.
  • Jensen GL. Inflammation: roles in aging and sarcopenia. JPEN J Parenter Enteral Nutr. 2008;32(6):656–9. doi:10.1177/0148607108324585.
  • Tolea MI, Chrisphonte S, Galvin JE. Sarcopenic obesity and cognitive performance. Clin Interv Aging. 2018;13:1111–9. doi:10.2147/CIA.S164113.
  • Batsis JA, Haudenschild C, Roth RM, Gooding TL, Roderka MN, Masterson T, Brand J, Lohman MC, Mackenzie TA. Incident impaired cognitive function in sarcopenic obesity: data from the national health and aging trends survey. J Am Med Dir Assoc. 2021;22(4):865–72.e865. doi:10.1016/j.jamda.2020.09.008.
  • Cavazzotto TG, de Campos CDV, Mazur CE, da Silva DF, Valério JMS, Vieira ER, da Silva W, Bonini JS. Association between cognitive performance and sarcopenic obesity in older adults with Alzheimer’s disease. Dement Neuropsychol. 2022;16(1):28–32. doi:10.1590/1980-5764-DN-2021-0039.
  • Levine M, Crimmins E. Sarcopenic obesity and cognitive functioning: the mediating roles of insulin resistance and inflammation? Curr Gerontol Geriatr Res. 2012;2012:826398. doi:10.1155/2012/826398.
  • Someya Y, Tamura Y, Kaga H, Sugimoto D, Kadowaki S, Suzuki R, Aoki S, Hattori N, Motoi Y, Shimada K, et al. Sarcopenic obesity is associated with cognitive impairment in community-dwelling older adults: the Bunkyo health study. Clin Nutr. 2022;41(5):1046–51. doi:10.1016/j.clnu.2022.03.017.
  • Wang H, Hai S, Liu YX, Cao L, Liu Y, Liu P, Yang Y, Dong BR. Associations between sarcopenic obesity and cognitive impairment in elderly Chinese community-dwelling individuals. J Nutr Health Aging. 2019;23(1):14–20. doi:10.1007/s12603-018-1088-3.
  • Yu PC, Hsu CC, Lee WJ, Liang CK, Chou MY, Lin MH, Hsiao FY, Peng LN, Chen LK. Muscle-to-fat ratio identifies functional impairments and cardiometabolic risk and predicts outcomes: biomarkers of sarcopenic obesity. J Cachexia Sarcopenia Muscle. 2022;13(1):368–76. doi:10.1002/jcsm.12877.
  • Baumgartner RN, Koehler KM, Gallagher D, Romero L, Heymsfield SB, Ross RR, Garry PJ, Lindeman RD. Epidemiology of sarcopenia among the elderly in New Mexico. Am J Epidemiol. 1998;147(8):755–63. doi:10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a009520.
  • Prado CM, Siervo M, Mire E, Heymsfield SB, Stephan BC, Broyles S, Smith SR, Wells JC, Katzmarzyk PT. A population-based approach to define body-composition phenotypes. Am J Clin Nutr. 2014;99(6):1369–77. doi:10.3945/ajcn.113.078576.
  • Siervo M, Prado CM, Mire E, Broyles S, Wells JC, Heymsfield S, Katzmarzyk PT. Body composition indices of a load–capacity model: gender-and BMI-specific reference curves. Public Health Nutr. 2015;18(7):1245–54. doi:10.1017/S1368980014001918.
  • Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Health Statistics. NHANES questionnaires, datasets, and related documentation. Hyattsville, MD: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention; 2024. https://wwwn.cdc.gov/nchs/nhanes/
  • Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Health Statistics. NCHS Ethics Review Board (ERB) approval. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention; 2024. https://www.cdc.gov/nchs/nhanes/irba98.htm
  • Nuttall FQ. Body mass index: obesity, BMI, and health: a critical review. Nutr Today. 2015;50(3):117–28. doi:10.1097/NT.0000000000000092.
  • World Health Organization. Waist circumference and waist-hip ratio: report of a who expert consultation. Geneva, 8–11 December 2008; 2011.
  • Petermann-Rocha F, Balntzi V, Gray SR, Lara J, Ho FK, Pell JP, Celis-Morales C. Global prevalence of sarcopenia and severe sarcopenia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Cachexia Sarcopenia Muscle. 2022;13(1):86–99. doi:10.1002/jcsm.12783.
  • Batsis JA, Barre LK, Mackenzie TA, Pratt SI, Lopez-Jimenez F, Bartels SJ. Variation in the prevalence of sarcopenia and sarcopenic obesity in older adults associated with different research definitions: dual-energy x-ray absorptiometry data from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey 1999–2004. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2013;61(6):974–80. doi:10.1111/jgs.12260.
  • Song W, Feng Y, Gong Z, Tian C. The association between dietary inflammatory index and cognitive performance in older adults aged 60 years and older. Front Nutr. 2022;9:748000. doi:10.3389/fnut.2022.748000.
  • Gong Z, Song W, Gu M, Zhou X, Tian C. Association between serum iron concentrations and cognitive impairment in older adults aged 60 years and older: a dose-response analysis of national health and nutrition examination survey. PLOS One. 2021;16(8):e0255595. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0255595.
  • Bailey RL, Jun S, Murphy L, Green R, Gahche JJ, Dwyer JT, Potischman N, McCabe GP, Miller JW. High folic acid or folate combined with low vitamin B-12 status: potential but inconsistent association with cognitive function in a nationally representative cross-sectional sample of us older adults participating in the NHANES. Am J Clin Nutr. 2020;112(6):1547–57. doi:10.1093/ajcn/nqaa239.
  • Zou D, Lloyd JE, Baumbusch JL. Using SPSS to analyze complex survey data: a primer. J Mod Appl Stat Methods. 2020;18(1):2–22. doi:10.22237/jmasm/1556670300.
  • Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. National Center for Health Statistics. Module 3: weighting. In: Module 3: weighting. Hyattsville, MD: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention; 2024. https://wwwn.cdc.gov/nchs/nhanes/tutorials/weighting.aspx
  • Preacher KJ, Hayes AF. Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behav Res Methods. 2008;40(3):879–91. doi:10.3758/brm.40.3.879.
  • Mazidi M, Katsiki N, Kengne AP, Mikhailidis DP, Banach M. Adiposity mediates the association between whole grain consumption, glucose homeostasis and insulin resistance: findings from the US NHANES. Lipids Health Dis. 2018;17(1):219. doi:10.1186/s12944-018-0805-6.
  • Hou Q, Guan Y, Yu W, Liu X, Wu L, Xiao M, Lü Y. Associations between obesity and cognitive impairment in the Chinese elderly: an observational study. Clin Interv Aging. 2019;14:367–73. doi:10.2147/CIA.S192050.
  • Van Gaal LF, Mertens IL, De Block CE. Mechanisms linking obesity with cardiovascular disease. Nature. 2006;444(7121):875–80. doi:10.1038/nature05487.
  • Liang F, Fu J, Moore JB, Zhang X, Xu Y, Qiu N, Wang Y, Li R. Body mass index, waist circumference, and cognitive decline among Chinese older adults: a nationwide retrospective cohort study. Front Aging Neurosci. 2022;14:737532. doi:10.3389/fnagi.2022.737532.
  • West RK, Ravona-Springer R, Heymann A, Schmeidler J, Leroith D, Koifman K, D’Arcy RC, Song X, Guerrero-Berroa E, Preiss R, et al. Waist circumference is correlated with poorer cognition in elderly type 2 diabetes women. Alzheimers Dement. 2016;12(8):925–9. doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2016.03.017.
  • Sakib MN, Ramezan R, Thompson ME, Best JR, Hall PA. Cognitive function is associated with multiple indices of adiposity in the Canadian longitudinal study on aging: a cross-sectional analysis. Psychosom Med. 2022;84(7):773–84. doi:10.1097/PSY.0000000000001099.
  • Anand SS, Friedrich MG, Lee DS, Awadalla P, Després JP, Desai D, de Souza RJ, Dummer T, Parraga G, Larose E, et al. Evaluation of adiposity and cognitive function in adults. JAMA Netw Open. 2022;5(2):e2146324. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2021.46324.
  • Morys F, Dadar M, Dagher A. Obesity impairs cognitive function via metabolic syndrome and cerebrovascular disease: an SEM analysis in 15,000 adults from the UK biobank. bioRxiv. 2020;2020.2006.2026.174086.
  • Luchsinger JA, Biggs ML, Kizer JR, Barzilay J, Fitzpatrick A, Newman A, Longstreth WT, Lopez O, Siscovick D, Kuller L. Adiposity and cognitive decline in the cardiovascular health study. Neuroepidemiology. 2013;40(4):274–81. doi:10.1159/000345136.
  • Seo YK, Won CW, Soh Y. Associations between body composition and cognitive function in an elderly Korean population: a cohort-based cross-sectional study. Medicine. 2021;100(9):e25027. doi:10.1097/MD.0000000000025027.
  • Bagger YZ, Tankó LB, Alexandersen P, Qin G, Christiansen C. The implications of body fat mass and fat distribution for cognitive function in elderly women. Obes Res. 2004;12(9):1519–26. doi:10.1038/oby.2004.189.
  • Dye L, Boyle NB, Champ C, Lawton C. The relationship between obesity and cognitive health and decline. Proc Nutr Soc. 2017;76(4):443–54. doi:10.1017/S0029665117002014.
  • Tessier A-J, Wing SS, Rahme E, Morais JA, Chevalier S. Association of low muscle mass with cognitive function during a 3-year follow-up among adults aged 65 to 86 years in the Canadian longitudinal study on aging. JAMA Netw Open. 2022;5(7):e2219926. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2022.19926.
  • Sui SX, Williams LJ, Holloway-Kew KL, Hyde NK, Leach S, Pasco JA. Associations between muscle quality and cognitive function in older men: cross-sectional data from the Geelong osteoporosis study. J Clin Densitom. 2022;25(2):133–40. doi:10.1016/j.jocd.2021.03.007.
  • Sui SX, Holloway-Kew KL, Hyde NK, Williams LJ, Tembo MC, Mohebbi M, Gojanovic M, Leach S, Pasco JA. Handgrip strength and muscle quality in Australian women: cross-sectional data from the Geelong osteoporosis study. J Cachexia Sarcopenia Muscle. 2020;11(3):690–7. doi:10.1002/jcsm.12544.
  • Tou NX, Wee SL, Pang BWJ, Lau LK, Jabbar KA, Seah WT, Chen KK, Ng TP. Associations of fat mass and muscle function but not lean mass with cognitive impairment: the Yishun study. PLOS One. 2021;16(8):e0256702. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0256702.
  • Sui SX, Balanta-Melo J, Pasco JA, Plotkin LI. Musculoskeletal deficits and cognitive impairment: epidemiological evidence and biological mechanisms. Curr Osteoporos Rep. 2022;20(5):260–72. doi:10.1007/s11914-022-00736-9.
  • Lopez-Lopez JP, Cohen DD, Ney-Salazar D, Martinez D, Otero J, Gomez-Arbelaez D, Camacho PA, Sanchez-Vallejo G, Arcos E, Narvaez C, et al. The prediction of metabolic syndrome alterations is improved by combining waist circumference and handgrip strength measurements compared to either alone. Cardiovasc Diabetol. 2021;20(1):68. doi:10.1186/s12933-021-01256-z.
  • Low S, Goh KS, Ng TP, Ang SF, Moh A, Wang J, Ang K, Subramaniam T, Sum CF, Lim SC. The prevalence of sarcopenic obesity and its association with cognitive performance in type 2 diabetes in Singapore. Clin Nutr. 2020;39(7):2274–81. doi:10.1016/j.clnu.2019.10.019.
  • Merchant RA, Seetharaman S, Au L, Wong MWK, Wong BLL, Tan LF, Chen MZ, Ng SE, Soong JTY, Hui RJY, et al. Relationship of fat mass index and fat free mass index with body mass index and association with function, cognition and sarcopenia in pre-frail older adults. Front Endocrinol. 2021;12:765415. doi:10.3389/fendo.2021.765415.
  • de Luca C, Olefsky JM. Inflammation and insulin resistance. FEBS Lett. 2008;582(1):97–105. doi:10.1016/j.febslet.2007.11.057.
  • Zhong Y, Ya M, Jia WP, Hong Y, By W, Hyperinsulinemia JJ. Insulin resistance and cognitive decline in older cohort. Biomed Environ Sci. 2012;25(1):8–14.
  • Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, Williams JB. The PHQ-9: validity of a brief depression severity measure. J Gen Intern Med. 2001;16(9):606–13. doi:10.1046/j.1525-1497.2001.016009606.x.
  • Brooks JM, Titus A, Bruce M, Orzechowski N, Mackenzie T, Bartels S, Batsis J. Depression and handgrip strength among us adults aged 60 years and older from NHANES 2011–2014. J Nutr Health Aging. 2018;22(8):938–43. doi:10.1007/s12603-018-1041-5.
  • Jaeger J. Digit symbol substitution test: the case for sensitivity over specificity in neuropsychological testing. J Clin Psychopharmacol. 2018;38(5):513–9. doi:10.1097/JCP.0000000000000941.