117
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
REVIEW

Resilience to Stress and Adversity: A Narrative Review of the Role of Positive Affect

ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon
Pages 2011-2038 | Received 18 Dec 2023, Accepted 23 Apr 2024, Published online: 14 May 2024

Abstract

The modern conception of mental health encompasses not only mental illness but also mental wellbeing, including positive emotional states and other forms of positive experience. Accordingly, research on resilience — that is, recovery or adaptation following adversity – has recently expanded to consider the roles of positive affect in the resilience process. To review this research, we performed a keyword search of all peer-reviewed journals within the American Psychological Association’s PsycInfo database, retrieving all studies of positive affect in the context of resilience. These studies measured positive affect either as the outcome of the resilience process or as a resilience resource in its own right. With positive affect as the outcome, the literature suggests that various resilience resources can promote positive affect following a stressor, especially positive personality traits (eg, hope, optimism, self-compassion) and supportive interpersonal connections. With positive affect as a resilience resource, the literature suggests that higher levels of positive affect may protect individuals from the impact of stress on a number of outcomes, such as depression and trauma symptoms. In all, the reviewed research showcases a wide range of stressors, resources, and outcomes, and there are numerous openings for future discoveries in this promising area of inquiry.

Introduction

Historical Context

Until recent decades, mental health was measured and studied primarily in terms of mental illness, negative emotional states, and other forms of psychological distress or dysfunction, along with the risk factors that predict or exacerbate such conditions. But more recently, mental health has been increasingly recognised as comprising two related but distinct domains, namely mental illness and mental wellbeing.Citation1–4 Thus, the modern conception of mental health acknowledges not only negative symptoms but also positive outcomes, and with this broadened view have come a range of studies on protective factors that predict or accentuate favourable mental health trajectories. Consequently, there is now a large literature on resilience, and the various attributes that support positive mental functioning in the aftermath of stress, trauma, and other forms of adversity. Initially, even studies of protective factors examined them mostly in relation to negative outcomes (eg, studies of personality traits that may predict lower levels of depression), but more recent research has considered predictors of positive attributes too, especially positive affect and related variables (eg, happiness, subjective wellbeing). The overlapping fields of resilience and positive affect have produced numerous studies that shed light on how resilience can be both enabled by positive affect and measured in terms of it, and the present paper offers a narrative review of these connections.

Defining Resilience

The concept of resilience has been defined and studied in various ways, and we must begin by clarifying it for the present purposes. Across all definitions of resilience, there is reference to the phenomenon of regaining (if not maintaining or even increasing) one’s mental health following a stressor (eg, a challenge, hardship, or aversive experience).Citation5 Here we are using the broad scientific definition of “stressor” (ie, any experience that involves a challenge, threat, or hardship), as well as the broadest definition of “mental health”, encompassing all aspects – positive and negative – of an individual’s psychological health and functioning. Therefore, studying resilience requires investigations into why some people rebound from or adapt to stressors more easily than others, or experience stressors as less challenging or distressing from the outset, and why certain individuals demonstrate improvements in their mental health even after highly stressful or traumatic experiences (eg, posttraumatic growthCitation6). In addressing these research questions, a recent systematic review of resilience interventions notes that resilience has been operationally defined in one of three ways: as an outcome, as a process, and as a trait.Citation7 The outcome approach defines resilience as favourable mental outcomes at a specified point in time following the stressor in question (eg, lower levels of anxiety and higher levels of wellbeing three months after suffering a violent crime).Citation7 The process approach defines resilience in a similar way, but with multiple measurement occasions (either before and after the stressor, or at multiple timepoints following the stressor), allowing one to identify different trajectories of mental functioning following the stressor (eg, temporarily versus indefinitely reduced wellbeing after a major loss).Citation7 In contrast to the outcome and process definitions, the trait approach emphasises the personality traits that define one’s ability to recover from or adapt to stressors.Citation7 The present review draws upon studies that employ all three definitions of resilience. However, we note that using the term “resilience” to label a trait (or an ensemble of traits) can make it difficult to differentiate the phenomenon of resilience (ie, recovery from or adaptation to stressors) from its predictors (such as the various protective traits that some researchers label as resilience). Therefore, we will use the term “resilience” to refer to the outcome or process of recovery or adaptation following a stressor, and we will use the phrase “trait resilience” where other authors have used the term to refer to personal attributes that enable the phenomenon of resilience to occur.

As stated above, all definitions of psychological resilience pertain to regaining or maintaining (or even improving) one’s mental health (including both positive and negative outcomes) in the wake of a stressor. Therefore, resilience cannot be directly studied outside the context of stressors (ie, a challenge, threat, or hardship). There are many studies of trait resilience that examine this construct as a predictor, outcome, mediator, or moderator within models that do not contain any measure of perceived or actual stress, challenge, or adversity (eg, models that correlate trait resilience measures with other positive factors, such as wellbeing or cognitive performance). Such studies may be valuable in elucidating the complex interplay of the psychological variables in question, but they do not test whether the characteristics labelled as “resilience” do in fact mitigate the impact of stressors. Therefore, the present review considers only those studies of resilience that examine it in relation to some kind of stressor (moreover, it would be beyond the scope of the review to employ a more expansive usage of the term “resilience”). In this framework, a protective factor (or resilience resource) is necessarily one that moderates the impact of a stressor on one’s mental health.

The Present Review

With these definitional matters resolved, we turn to the subject of our review: the relationship between resilience and positive affect. In this context, the dual role of positive affect is evident from the very definition of resilience. Given that resilience refers to mental health recovery or adaptation, and that mental health involves not only negative symptoms but also positive outcomes, then clearly positive affect can be treated as a measure of resilience. Therefore, all else being equal, those who experience smaller declines in positive emotion following a stressor, and those whose positive affect returns more quickly or more fully to pre-stressor levels, have exhibited greater resilience. Likewise, in the context of adaptation (rather than recovery), those who experience greater increases in positive affect following a challenge or stressor have exhibited greater resilience. Thus, the outcome or process of resilience can be measured in terms of positive affect, as well as other factors whose measures incorporate positive affect (eg, subjective wellbeing, life satisfaction). Alternatively, positive affect can be treated as a resilience resource – a personal or psychosocial attribute that may enable resilience. In this approach, those with greater pre-stressor positive affect are hypothesised to be more resistant to the stressor or better able to recover from (or adapt to) it; in other words, positive affect enables resilience by moderating the impact of the stressor (specifically, by mitigating this impact). The present review acknowledges both roles that positive affect can play in the context of resilience; therefore, it includes studies that treat positive affect either as an outcome measure of resilience or as a resilience resource.

Having clarified both the concept of resilience and dual role of positive affect in relation to it, we may now begin the review proper. The purpose of our review was to summarise the findings from all studies of psychological resilience (within a relevant database) that involved positive affect in one of these two roles. Therefore, the review is divided into two main sections, with the first section covering positive affect as an outcome measure of resilience and the second section covering positive affect as a resilience resource (see ). Each section is itself divided into subsections according to the stressor or adversity involved, allowing researchers and practitioners to see which forms of hardship have already been studied and which ones may warrant greater attention. Although we did not seek to follow the format of a systematic review, we still conducted our literature search in a rigorous manner. We performed a keyword search (the broadest mode of search) of all peer-reviewed journals in the American Psychological Association’s PsycInfo database, using the following search terms: (resilien* AND (“positive affect” OR “positive emotion*” OR “positive mood” OR “positive feeling*” OR positivity OR wellbeing OR “well-being” OR “well being” OR wellness OR happy OR happi* OR “life satisfaction”)). Thus, the search terms covered all words derived from resilience (eg, resilient, resiliency) and all words or phrases that either are synonymous with positive affect (or treated as such by many researchers) or denote variables (eg, wellbeing) whose definitions and/or measures often incorporate positive affect in some way. From the results of this search, we identified every empirical, quantitative study that measured resilience directly (ie, as the moderation of the impact of a stressor on a given mental health outcome), including studies whose tests of moderation yielded null findings. Our synthesis of these studies comprises the body of this review. From this synthesis, we have aimed to identify the resilience resources that promote the outcome of positive affect following a stressor, as well as the ways in which positive affect can serve as a resilience resource in its own right.

Figure 1 Graphical summary of review structure. The current review focuses on the overall topic of “resilience and positive affect” which can be considered in two ways, with “positive affect as an outcome measure” (left-hand model) or “positive affect as a resilience resource” (right-hand model). Each model is independent and depicts the phenomenon of resilience, whereby the impact of a stressor on a mental health outcome is moderated by a resilience resource. In the left-hand model, the outcome variable is positive affect. In the right-hand model, the moderator variable is positive affect. Thus, the models illustrate the two roles that positive affect may play within the resilience process.

Figure 1 Graphical summary of review structure. The current review focuses on the overall topic of “resilience and positive affect” which can be considered in two ways, with “positive affect as an outcome measure” (left-hand model) or “positive affect as a resilience resource” (right-hand model). Each model is independent and depicts the phenomenon of resilience, whereby the impact of a stressor on a mental health outcome is moderated by a resilience resource. In the left-hand model, the outcome variable is positive affect. In the right-hand model, the moderator variable is positive affect. Thus, the models illustrate the two roles that positive affect may play within the resilience process.

Positive Affect as an Outcome Measure of Resilience

Of the studies retrieved from our search, most pertained to positive affect as an outcome (ie, as a measure of resilience) rather than a resilience resource. This is unsurprising, because while affective states can certainly motivate action, they are typically studied as responses to antecedent thoughts, beliefs, or experiences. This is especially so in the mental health context, where affective states are frequently cited in describing and diagnosing various psychological conditions. Positive affect specifically has been studied as a response to numerous kinds of stressor, which we have divided into six categories. The most traumatic stressors (usually involving abuse, violent crime, or other forms of severe mistreatment) are placed in two categories, one encompassing adverse childhood experiences and the other encompassing traumatic experiences in adulthood. More common stressors (eg, daily hassles, everyday challenges, personal disputes) are placed in another two categories, one covering occupational stressors (including stressors not only in the workplace but also in academic and childrearing contexts) and the other covering experiences that are typically grouped under general labels such as “stressful life events”, “recent life events”, or “everyday stressors”. It should be noted that the events labelled in such ways are not necessarily minor; they can include events that are not usually considered to be traumatic or egregious but can still be significantly upsetting or distressing (eg, having a serious argument, losing one’s job), and some studies of stressful life events combine measures of less severe stressors with measures of potentially traumatic events (PTEs). The final two categories include adversity related to medical illnesses or other health conditions, with one category dedicated to COVID-19-related stressors (given the number of studies on this topic) and the other category covering all other medical or health conditions. Across all six categories, each study includes at least one outcome measure that either measures positive affect exclusively or incorporates positive affect along with other aspects of mental health or wellbeing. See for a summary of these studies and the key variables in each one.

Table 1 Summary of Reviewed Studies on Positive Affect as an Outcome of the Resilience Process

Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACEs)

Our search identified seventeen studies that examined resilience in terms of the effects of ACEs (occurring during childhood or adolescence) on positive affect. In this context, the most commonly studied resilience resources are internal ones, especially traits. Three studies have investigated the role of trait resilience specifically, with mixed results. In a study of adolescents residing in the Netherlands, Sleijpen et alCitation19 found that trait resilience moderated the relationship between PTEs (eg, loss of parent, abuse, lack of shelter) and satisfaction with life in a sample of 148 non-refugee secondary school students but not in a sample of 117 refugees residing in asylum seeker centres (who had experienced a significantly higher number and variety of PTEs than the non-refugees). In the non-refugees, number of PTEs was negatively associated with satisfaction with life, with this association being non-significant in those with above-average levels of trait resilience and increasingly negative with increasingly below-average levels of trait resilience. The absence of a similar moderation effect in the refugees suggests that individual differences in trait resilience may not always be enough to moderate the effects of the PTEs typically experienced by asylum seekers, who are at a markedly higher level of risk and experience different stressors from those typically encountered by non-refugees. The other two studies investigated trait resilience in relation to retrospectively reported ACEs. In a sample of 255 Korean unwed mothers, Kang et alCitation13 found that a higher number of recalled ACEs was associated with greater perceived discrimination, which in turn was associated with lower wellbeing, especially among those with lower trait resilience. In addition, in a study of 98 Australia-based university students, Tung et alCitation23 divided the participants into three categories: control (few ACEs, low depression and anxiety), vulnerable (more ACEs, low trait resilience), and resilient (more ACEs, high trait resilience). The vulnerable group (who had lower trait resilience) reported lower positive affect than the control group and higher negative affect than the other two groups, while the resilient group (who had higher trait resilience) did not differ from the control group in positive affect.

Five studies have examined internal resilience resources other than trait resilience. Cohen et alCitation11 used clustering analyses to divide 584 US adolescents into distinct profiles, representing differing levels of psychological distress and mental wellbeing in response to previous experiences of PTEs (community violence and emotional maltreatment). Participants who had both high wellbeing and low distress (accounting for PTE exposure) reported the highest levels of trait emotional intelligence. Moreover, among those who had low wellbeing, emotional intelligence was higher in those who reported low (rather than high) distress. In a study of over 6000 US adults, Turiano et alCitation24 found that perceived control beliefs moderated the association between recalled childhood misfortune and current affective outcomes, with childhood misfortune predicting lower positive affect and greater negative affect more strongly among those with a lower level of perceived control over their lives. Similarly, Seon and Smith-AdcockCitation18 found a buffering effect of meaning in life in a nationally representative sample of over 4800 US 15-year-olds, whereby the negative association between bullying victimisation (via verbal, physical, or relational bullying) and life satisfaction was weaker among those with a greater sense of life meaning. Likewise, in a sample of 454 Polish adolescents, Riley et alCitation17 found that problem-solving coping strategies (but not social support-seeking or avoidant strategies) significantly moderated the negative relationship between victimisation and emotional wellbeing. Those who had experienced more types of victimisation (across categories such as bullying, crime, and child maltreatment) reported lower life satisfaction and lower positive affect, but less so if they reported greater use of problem-solving coping. Coping strategies were also examined by Sorek et alCitation22 in a study of 122 Israeli children of divorced parents. It was found that child-rated parental conflict was associated with lower life satisfaction only among children with low levels of active coping. However, this moderation effect was observed for only one of the two measures of life satisfaction employed by the authors, and such effects were not observed when happiness or psychological wellbeing were the outcomes. When mother-rated parental conflict was considered, there was only one interaction effect, with reduced life satisfaction (on only one of the two measures) only in children with high self-blame.

A further five studies have examined the role of external resilience resources, specifically social or interpersonal connections. In an additional analysis from the aforementioned study of children of divorced parents, SorekCitation21 found that parental conflict was associated with lower life satisfaction only among children with less-close relationships with their grandparents. However, this interaction effect was found for only one of the two child-rated measures of parental conflict (feeling caught between one’s parents), and it was not found when happiness or psychological wellbeing was the outcome. DiClemente et alCitation12 examined the effects of violence exposure on the positive affect of 269 Black American adolescents, finding no evidence that family cohesion, neighbourhood cohesion, or school cohesion moderated the relationship between violence exposure (over the previous 12 months) and current positive affect in cross-sectional analyses of the participants in the seventh and eighth grades. In fact, violence exposure had no main effect on positive affect in either grade. In longitudinal analyses, violence exposure in seventh grade negatively predicted positive affect in eighth grade (controlling for seventh grade positive affect), and this relationship was moderated by family cohesion (but not neighbourhood or school cohesion), with the violence-affect relationship becoming less negative with higher levels of cohesion. However, this longitudinal result was found only in boys, and while the authors tested innumerable two-way and three-way interaction effects, they did not report any correction of the significance threshold to account for multiple testing. More straightforward results were obtained by Nurius et alCitation15, who investigated retrospectively reported ACEs in a sample of over 13,000 US adults. The authors found a weaker negative correlation between number of recalled ACEs and perceived wellbeing among those with a greater sense of community, referring to adequate social support and satisfaction with one’s neighbourhood. External resilience resources have also been examined in the context of adolescent adversity related to ethnic or cultural differences. In a study of 247 British secondary school students of South Asian descent, Bagci et alCitation9 found that perceived ethnic discrimination was negatively related to psychological wellbeing only among those with fewer cross-ethnic friendships. In addition, in a study of 524 US adolescents of Mexican descent,Citation16 bicultural stress (ie, stress related to maintaining one’s heritage culture while adapting to the surrounding culture) had a stronger negative association with life satisfaction among those with stronger connections to their female caregiver (while male caregiver connectedness did not moderate this association). This may have been due to a floor effect, because those with lower female caregiver connectedness had lower life satisfaction even at low levels of bicultural stress.

Apart from research on internal and external resilience resources, four studies have considered physiological or physical health factors in the context of resilience to ACEs. In a study of over 3000 US adults, Nishimi et alCitation14 divided the participants into four categories: resilient (adversity-exposed, good mental health), non-resilient (adversity-exposed, poor mental health), positive functioning (not adversity-exposed, good mental health), and unfavourable functioning (not adversity-exposed, poor mental health). These groups were then compared in terms of physical health behaviours (non-smoking, moderate alcohol consumption, regular exercise, healthy nutrition) and body weight. The resilient group did not differ from the positive functioning group except on non-smoking (there being significantly more non-smokers in the latter group). Furthermore, the resilient group was significantly healthier than the non-resilient group in terms of physical activity, nutrition, and body weight. Two studies have employed cardiac measures as indicators of resilience resources, given that emotional and behavioural regulation have been linked with variables such as respiratory sinus arrhythmia and heart rate. In a study of 92 US preschool children, Cipriano et alCitation10 investigated whether vagal suppression moderated the relationship between violence exposure and three outcome variables (emotional problems, frustration, and positive affect), finding an interaction effect on emotional problems only. In contrast, in a sample of 150 US undergraduate students with a history of childhood maltreatment, Somers et alCitation20 found a moderating effect of heart rate reactivity, whereby maltreatment was negatively associated with positive affect in those with above-average or average heart rate reactivity, but not in those with below-average reactivity. One key difference between the Cipriano et alCitation10 and Somers et alCitation20 studies (aside from the different cardiac measures employed) lies in their sample demographics, as the former recruited children with current or recent adversity whereas the latter employed an adult sample with recalled ACEs. Lastly, in a study of over 4800 UK adults, Armitage et alCitation8 employed polygenic scores for wellbeing and depression (ie, genetic correlates of mental health) as potential moderators of the effect of peer victimisation at age 13 on mental wellbeing at age 23. The authors found no evidence of moderation, leading them to conclude that genetic profiling may not be useful for identifying those who are more or less resilient to bullying.

Together, studies of the effects of ACE on positive affect have shown that internal resilience resources such as trait resilience and external resources such as social support may serve to mitigate the negative impact of ACEs in both childhood and adulthood, although the evidence is mixed in some areas. There is also limited research involving physiological indices of resilience, with evidence that physical and cardiovascular health factors may be influential.

Adult Experiences of Mistreatment or Trauma

When shifting the lens to adult experiences of adversity and their effects on positive affect, our search yielded only four studies of resilience, all of which examined internal resilience resources. Two of the studies focused on minority populations at greater risk of suffering discrimination. In a sample of 242 Native American and Alaskan undergraduates, Fetter et alCitation25 examined a culturally specific stressor (perceived historical loss) as a predictor of mental wellbeing. The results showed a significant interaction effect whereby there was a weaker negative relationship between historical loss and wellbeing among those with a stronger sense of ethnic identity (although this resource did not moderate the effect of historical loss on psychological distress). Similarly, in a study of 872 Chinese impoverished college students, Liu et alCitation27 found a weaker negative association between perceived discrimination and subjective wellbeing among those with higher levels of belief in a just world (ie, the belief that the world is ultimately fair). This study also found that trait resilience moderated the impact of discrimination on self-esteem, but not as expected: Those with higher trait resilience had a larger self-esteem advantage at lower rather than higher levels of discrimination, possibly indicating a floor effect.

The other two studies examined stressors related to conflict or mistreatment. In a sample of over 1100 Spanish university students, Yubero et alCitation28 found a weaker negative relationship between chronic bullying victimisation (at school and university) and psychological wellbeing among those with higher levels of trait resilience, suggesting a buffering effect. Finally, Lee et alCitation26 employed a clustering analysis of posttraumatic stress symptoms and mental wellbeing in a sample of 367 South Korean male veterans of the Vietnam War, which yielded five distinct classes, including groups labelled “resilient”, “moderate distress”, and “severe distress”. The resilient group, who reported the highest levels of wellbeing, scored higher than the other groups on a range of resilience resources, including optimism, positive appraisals of military service, and social support.

Stressful Life Events or Everyday Stressors

Our search returned twenty-one studies that examined resilience in terms of the effects of stressful life events on positive affect, with the majority investigating how these effects may be moderated by internal resilience resources such as personality traits. Two studies have examined trait resilience specifically, with mixed findings. In a study of 95 Mexican informal caregivers, Ertl et alCitation36 observed a weaker correlation between perceived stress over the past month and mental quality of life (but not physical health-related quality of life) among those with higher trait resilience. In contrast, in a study of 486 Chinese rural-to-urban migrant school students, Huang et alCitation42 found that family socioeconomic status (SES) was positively correlated with life satisfaction only in those whose participating parent was low in positive affect. The children’s own levels of trait resilience did not moderate the relationship between SES and life satisfaction (although trait resilience moderated the relationship between SES and emotion regulation).

Five studies have looked at personality traits other than trait resilience. In a longitudinal study of 797 community adults across 42 countries, Goodman et alCitation38 found that higher trait hope at each timepoint predicted a weaker negative association between stressful life events and subjective wellbeing over the following three months. However, such interaction effects were not observed when other traits, including grit, meaning in life, curiosity, gratitude, perceived control, and use of personal strengths, were analysed as moderators. In another longitudinal study, involving over 1100 Greek university students, Arampatzi et alCitation30 found that those with more positive expectations at baseline experienced smaller drops and larger rebounds in happiness over the subsequent timepoints, which coincided with major socioeconomic upheaval in the target population (the Greek bailout referendum). Similarly, in a nationwide study of over 15,000 Australian adults,Citation31 the negative associations between positive affect and both neighbourhood crime levels and being a victim of crime were smaller among those with a more internal locus of control (ie, those who perceived themselves as being more in control of the events in their lives). Additionally, in a study of 235 US adults who identified as LGBQ (lesbian, gay, bisexual, and/or queer), Chong et alCitation33 found that daily negative experiences related to the participant’s sexual orientation were associated with lower positive affect, but not in those with high self-compassion (these experiences were also associated with higher negative affect, but this relationship was not moderated by self-compassion). Furthermore, in a laboratory study of 130 US university students, Corral-Frías et alCitation34 found that an experimentally contrived stressor resulted in reduced positive affect and increased negative affect in participants, but those with higher reward sensitivity reported smaller decreases in positive affect.

Three studies have examined cognitive reappraisal (an emotion regulation strategy) or related practices as resilience resources. Hodzic et alCitation41 investigated emotional intelligence (labelled “trait meta-mood” by the authors) in a study of 835 university students across three countries (Spain, Portugal, and Brazil). Perceived stress over the past month was negatively correlated with life satisfaction, but less so among those with higher scores on a trait meta-mood subscale measuring “emotional repair” (ie, using positive thinking to overcome negative moods), a construct comparable with cognitive reappraisal. Furthermore, in a daily-diary study of 236 English undergraduate students, Johnson et alCitation44 found that greater daily stressors were associated with lower positive mood and greater negative mood, with the latter association being weaker among those who were higher in cognitive reappraisal and lower in brooding (a rumination style that contrasts with reflection). However, the relationship between daily stressors and positive mood was not moderated by any of the emotion regulation strategies or rumination styles under study. Conversely, in an experimental intervention involving 83 Canadian undergraduates, Philippe et alCitation48 observed smaller reductions in positive emotion when participants had been randomly allocated to a 10-minute resilience intervention (guided cognitive reappraisal) prior to a negative mood induction, rather than one of the control conditions (neutral music or guided relaxation).

Five studies have assessed external resilience resources, especially resources related to social or interpersonal factors. Two of these studies examined moderators of the relationship between excessive use of digital technology (a common source of everyday stress) and positive affect. Jagtiani et alCitation43 assessed the use of social networking sites (SNS) in a nationally representative sample of over 2200 UK young adults aged 16–21 who still lived with their parents. There was a significant interaction effect whereby heavy SNS use was associated with low mental wellbeing primarily among those who did not share any evening meals with their family. Mutz et alCitation46 evaluated the subjective wellbeing of 76 adolescents from German-speaking countries before and after they participated in 10 days of outdoor group recreational activities (eg, hiking, climbing, canoeing) in Southern France. For those who reported ≤3 hours per day of screen time (ie, leisure time spent in front of a television or computer), life satisfaction was high prior to the outdoor program and did not significantly increase over time, but for those who reported >3 hours of screen time per day, life satisfaction was initially low but then increased significantly. However, this moderation effect was not observed for hedonic balance (ie, positive affect minus negative affect), with hedonic balance increasing over time regardless of baseline screen time levels. It should also be noted that the resilience resource in this study (outdoor group recreation) comprised not only social interaction but also physical activity and exposure to natural environments, which may bring their own benefits with regard to resilience.

The other three studies of external resources also returned mixed results. In a nationally representative sample of 373 US older adults, Greenfield and MarksCitation39 measured the stressor of “major role-identity absences”, referring to a lack of important interpersonal roles (ie, among those without a job, spouse, or child). Greater role-identity absences were associated with a lower sense of purpose in life only among those who did not engage in monthly volunteering, but such interaction effects were not found when the outcome was either positive or negative affect. Furthermore, in a study of 726 Athenian high school students, Hatzichristou et alCitation40 found that everyday life difficulties (due to the contemporaneous Greek economic recession) were negatively correlated with joy in life and self-esteem, but these negative associations were lessened by positive peer relations only among those students who were academic high-achievers. Similarly, in a study of 159 US adolescents with a mobility disability,Citation29 Alriksson-Schmidt et al found that everyday life stress (including disability-related stress) was negatively correlated with quality of life, but this association was not moderated by social competence, peer social engagement, or family functioning.

Two studies have examined multiple resilience resources, covering both internal and external factors. In a study of 197 Malaysian students from disadvantaged households, Noor and AlwiCitation47 divided the participants into three groups, labelled “adapted” (low life stress and high life satisfaction), “resilient” (high stress but high satisfaction), and “maladapted” (high stress and low satisfaction). The adapted and resilient students reported better mother-child communication and higher levels of support from their teachers; they also had higher scores on the Big Five personality traits (with Neuroticism reverse-scored) than the maladapted students. Similarly, in a nationwide study of almost 10,000 US adults, Bucknor and DerringerCitation32 found that life stress had a smaller negative association with positive affect among those who reported greater levels of social support; lower levels of loneliness; higher levels of subjective wellbeing; higher scores on the Big Five (with Neuroticism reverse-scored); higher scores on trait measures of optimism, mastery, purpose, and religiosity/spirituality; lower scores on pessimism and hopelessness; and higher levels of educational attainment. The same study also examined genetic data (European ancestries only), finding equivalent moderation effects with the polygenic scores for extraversion, neuroticism, subjective wellbeing, and educational attainment.

Six studies have examined whether the effects of life stress can be moderated by other stressful experiences. Three of these studies have suggested that previous adversity can lead to increased resilience against later stressors, provided the earlier adversity was not too severe. In a nationally representative study of over 2000 US adults, Seery et alCitation49 observed a curvilinear relationship between lifetime adversity and current life satisfaction, whereby satisfaction was highest among those who had experienced low levels of adversity, rather than no adversity or high levels of adversity. This study also found that lifetime adversity moderated the relationship between recent adversity (in the last 6 months) and life satisfaction, with recent adversity having the weakest negative association with life satisfaction among those who reported low levels of lifetime adversity. Similarly, in a study of 122 US women who had survived breast cancer, Dooley et alCitation35 found that the number of acute stressors in the one’s lifespan (pre-cancer diagnosis) predicted current levels of positive affect and cancer-related intrusive thoughts in a curvilinear fashion, whereby those who had experienced a moderate number of stressors (rather than a low or high number) reported greater positive affect and fewer intrusive thoughts. However, this study also found a straightforward linear relationship between acute stressors and negative affect, with greater stress predicting greater negative affect at a fixed rate. Mixed results were also obtained by Lazić et alCitation45 in a longitudinal study of 293 Serbian undergraduate students. The authors found that recent life stress was associated with lower life satisfaction in those who reported low or high but not moderate levels of perceived negative childhood experiences (PNCE); however, PNCE did not moderate the negative relationship between recent stress and positive affect (nor the positive relationship between recent stress and negative affect).

The other three studies have suggested that prior stressful experiences can compound rather than mitigate the effects of subsequent stressors. In a longitudinal experience-sampling study of 156 German adults, Gerstberger et alCitation37 found that current stress was negatively associated with current positive affect, and that this association was larger in those who reported greater ongoing chronic stress. This study also found that greater physical activity (since the last experience sample) predicted a weaker correlation between current stress and current negative affect (but not current positive affect), but this interaction was evident only for those who rated their past experiences of major life events as less severely stressful. In addition, the aforementioned studies by Nurius et alCitation15 and Tung et alCitation23 also shed light on the interaction between past and present stressors. Nurius et alCitation15 observed a stronger association between adult adversity and psychological distress among those who recalled greater childhood adversity, although this interaction effect was not found with perceived wellbeing as the outcome. Similarly, Tung et alCitation23 found that higher daily stress was associated with greater negative affect more strongly in the vulnerable group than in the resilient or control groups, but there was no such moderation of the association between daily stress and positive affect.

In summary, there is evidence that a range of internal and external resilience resources may mitigate the impact of everyday life stress on positive affect and related outcomes. Interestingly, a few studies have suggested that stressful experiences themselves can foster resilience against subsequent stressors, provided the earlier stress was not excessive. Such stress-inoculation effects have been found chiefly in the context of everyday stressors, presumably because more severe stressors are typically too harmful to yield net benefits (at least in terms of positive affect). However, there is also evidence that stressful experiences can compound the negative impact of later adversity, so the question of stress-inoculation clearly warrants continued investigation.

Occupational Stressors

Our search identified thirteen studies that examined resilience in terms of the effects of occupational stressors (including academic and childrearing stressors) on positive affect, with the majority investigating how this impact may be moderated by internal resilience resources, especially traits. Six studies (across five papers) have examined trait resilience specifically, with mixed results. In an experience sampling study of 65 full-time, guest-facing employees of US hotels, Shi et alCitation58 found that emotional dissonance (needing to show guests a different emotion from one’s actual feelings) was associated with lower positive affect, lower job satisfaction, and greater intention to leave the job only in those with low trait resilience (although there was no such interaction in relation to negative affect). In a longitudinal study of 57 US nurses, Gabriel et alCitation52 asked the participants to report the extent to which they had performed their patient-facing tasks (direct care) and other tasks (indirect care) to their satisfaction, after each of six consecutive shifts (thus, a lack of satisfaction with one’s caregiving constituted the occupational stressor). Direct care satisfaction predicted greater positive affect and lower negative affect (post-shift minus pre-shift), but neither of these associations was moderated by trait resilience or ratings of nurse-physician collegiality. However, while indirect care satisfaction did not have a main effect on positive affect, there were moderation effects whereby indirect care satisfaction predicted greater positive affect only at lower levels of trait resilience or collegiality (collegiality also moderated the association between indirect care satisfaction and negative affect). In a roleplay-based study of 66 Dutch paramedics, van Erp et alCitation60 had the participants respond to a simulated medical emergency involving interference from a bystander. Higher perceived interference was correlated with lower positive affect, both directly and indirectly via more-compromised cognition (ie, poorer concentration on the emergency), and the relationship between perceived interference and cognition was stronger in those with lower trait resilience. The authors published this roleplay-based study alongside a randomised experiment of bystander conflict in a university student setting. In this experiment, 47 Dutch students completed mathematical and grammatical exercises while a bystander was either silent or loud and rude. The latter condition resulted in lower positive affect, greater negative affect, and more unfavourable appraisals of the bystander, but only the effect on appraisals (not the effects on positive and negative affect) was especially strong among those with lower trait resilience. In another experimental study, of 337 German smartphone-owning employees, Lutz et alCitation55 had the participants react to imaginary scenarios wherein the work-home boundary was violated and they were pressured via text message to interrupt their current activity (by a friend/colleague requesting a call during/outside working hours). Higher pressure predicted greater negative affect but had no impact on positive affect, and trait resilience was not a significant moderator with either positive or negative affect as the outcome. In a study of 631 Ghanaian full-time employees, Annor and Amponsah-TawiahCitation50 found that workplace bullying was negatively associated with mental wellbeing more strongly among those with higher trait resilience. The authors found this result contradictory to the claim that trait resilience is protective, although the data indicate a possible floor effect, whereby trait resilience correlates with wellbeing but not at high levels of bullying (in which case wellbeing bottoms out for everyone).

Four studies have examined occupational stressors in the context of personality traits other than trait resilience. In a longitudinal study of over 1200 Swiss employees, Udayar et alCitation59 found that work stress at baseline positively predicted work stress 12 months later across all participants, but baseline work stress negatively predicted 12-month life satisfaction only in those with an “oversensitive” personality profile, characterised by lower levels of extraversion and conscientiousness, and higher levels of neuroticism. In a study of over 2500 Swiss students in secondary school or vocational training, Wepf et alCitation61 found that the experience of being a carer (for a relative or close friend with a health problem) was negatively correlated with mental wellbeing only in those with lower levels of “trait benefit-finding” (ie, the disposition to perceive positive changes following adversity). In a longitudinal study of 237 Hong Kong-based university students, Lai and MakCitation53 found that daily hassles (in the university student context) over a one-month period predicted lower mental wellbeing only among those with low baseline levels of optimism, self-esteem, and perceived control. In a study of over 1600 German secondary school and university students, Leipold et alCitation54 found that perceived stress related to one’s academic work was negatively correlated with subjective wellbeing, but less so among those who were higher in either meaning-focused coping (involving acceptance and positive reinterpretation) or social support-seeking (ie, seeking both instrumental and emotional support). A third coping style (problem-focused coping) did not moderate the stress-wellbeing relationship.

Two studies, both in the childrearing context, have examined external resilience resources (specifically, forms of social support) as moderators of the effects of occupational stressors. In one study, of 139 US licensed foster parents, ShardaCitation56 found that self-reported parenting stress was negatively correlated with wellbeing, but less so among those who reported higher levels of social support (received and given). In the other study, of 152 US kinship caregivers (ie, non-parental caregivers such as foster parents), Sharda et alCitation57 found that parenting stress was negatively correlated with psychosocial quality of life, but this relationship was not moderated by received social support.

A single study has shed light on whether the effects of occupational stress can be moderated by prior stressful experiences. In this study, Bäccman et alCitation51 assessed 129 Swedish marines before and after an anti-piracy naval deployment. Interestingly, those with prior experiences of combat-related stress had lower levels of positive affect and higher levels of negative affect than their colleagues before but not after the deployment. This finding was not reported as a test of moderation, and it might merely reflect regression to the mean, but it points to a possible stress-inoculation effect that may warrant further study.

In summary, a number of studies have found that trait resilience and other personality traits may mitigate the negative impact of occupational stressors on positive affect, but the evidence is limited and mixed with regard to the possible protective role of social support in this context. It would also be unsurprising if stress-inoculation played a role in this context, given that occupational stressors are typically similar in severity to everyday life stressors (ie, stressful but not traumatic).

Health Conditions (Other Than COVID-19)

Our search returned seven studies that examined resilience in terms of the effects of health conditions (other than COVID-19) on positive affect. Five of these studies focused on internal resilience resources. In a study of over 1800 British older adults, Windle et alCitation68 examined whether trait resilience significantly moderated the negative association between number of chronic physical illnesses and life satisfaction across four age groups (50–59, 60–69, 70–79, and 80–89 years). There was a significant moderation effect in each group except the youngest one, but the nature of the effect was not uniform. In the 60–69 and 70–79 year old groups, the association between illness and life satisfaction was more negative among those with lower trait resilience. However, in the 80–89 year old group, this association was steeper for those with higher trait resilience. This seemingly inconsistent finding may have been due to a floor effect, because those aged 80–89 with low trait resilience had low levels of life satisfaction even when they reported few chronic illnesses. In addition, in a US-based study of 94 adults with or without stuttering, Plexico et alCitation67 found that stuttering was associated with lower satisfaction with life scores at lower levels of trait resilience but not at higher levels of trait resilience; however, neither adaptive nor maladaptive coping style was found to moderate the negative association between stuttering status and satisfaction with life. In a longitudinal, nationally representative study of over 11,000 German adults aged 40 years and over, Hajek and KönigCitation64 assessed how favourably the participants rated their own health relative to others of the same age, and whether these health comparisons became more or less favourable over time. Among those whose comparisons worsened from “the same” to “much worse”, there was a corresponding reduction in life satisfaction and an increase in negative affect. However, the effect on life satisfaction was weaker among those with higher self-efficacy, while the effect on negative affect was weaker among those with higher optimism or higher self-efficacy. Furthermore, while there was no main effect of worsened health comparisons on positive affect, there was an interaction effect reflecting smaller reductions in positive affect among those with greater optimism or self-esteem. In a study of over 12,000 adults from the general Dutch population, Bos et alCitation62 found that self-defeating humour moderated the negative association between symptoms of depression, anxiety, and stress and subjective wellbeing, with a weaker association among those higher in this form of humour. However, no such interaction effects were found when other factors were analysed as moderators, including trait empathy, religious belonging, occupation, and three other styles of humour (affiliative, self-enhancing, and aggressive). A null result was also obtained by Kratz et alCitation65 in a longitudinal study of 122 US women with osteoarthritis and/or fibromyalgia. The authors found that pain acceptance did not moderate the negative association between weekly worst pain severity and weekly positive affect (although the positive relationship between pain severity and negative affect was weaker among those with greater pain acceptance).

Three studies have investigated external resilience resources as moderators of the impact of illness on positive affect, with mixed findings. In a sample of over 4000 Arizonan adults (weighted to be representative of the statewide adult population), Okun et alCitation66 investigated whether volunteering moderated the association between number of chronic physical health conditions and positive affect. Those with more chronic conditions reported both lower positive affect and lower trait resilience on average, but these deficits were significantly smaller among those who had engaged in volunteering in the previous 12 months (compared with those who had not). Furthermore, in the aforementioned study by Bos et alCitation62 it was found that for participants with a partner, psychological distress symptoms had a weaker negative association with subjective wellbeing. However, Delfabbro et alCitation63 investigated the moderating influence of interpersonal factors in a study of body image satisfaction versus dissatisfaction, with a sample of over 1200 Australian secondary schools students. A range of potential moderators, reflecting sociability and family functioning (eg, extraversion, neuroticism, relationship status, number of friends, family adjustment), were tested, but none significantly moderated the negative relationship between body image dissatisfaction and self-esteem. However, in a sub-analysis of only those who were dissatisfied with their physical appearance, participants above the 90th percentile on self-esteem were found to be more extraverted and have better-functioning families, relative to those below that threshold.

In summary, both internal and external resilience resources have been found to moderate the relationship between health conditions and positive affect, with most of the studies pertaining to personality traits and other internal attributes. However, this area of research has covered only a limited range of health conditions to date. There may be certain forms of physical or mental illness whose impact on positive affect is more (or less) amenable to mitigation by a given resilience resource. Clearly, further research is needed in this domain.

COVID-19

Our search identified nine studies on resilience in relation to the COVID-19 pandemic and its effects on positive affect. Almost all of these studies specified that data collection occurred under strict lockdown conditions (between the years 2020 and 2021). Given that individuals had little control over their external circumstances during the lockdowns, it is understandable that many of the studies focused on internal resilience resources (such as trait resilience or other traits). However, a few researchers examined how other factors may have served as resilience resources during the pandemic.

Six studies (across five papers) investigated internal resilience resources as moderators, two of which focused on trait resilience. In a study of 711 Spanish university students, Paredes et alCitation74 found that the perceived threat of COVID-19 was negatively associated with positive affect via heightened pandemic-related anxiety about the future, with this indirect negative effect being stronger among those with lower trait resilience. In addition, in a study of over 1000 Chinese university students, Hu et alCitation73 found that perceived stress had a stronger negative association with both social adaptation (to the pandemic) and life satisfaction among those with lower levels of trait resilience (called “emotional resilience” by the authors). However, Hu et al neglected to specify the names of the scales they employed to measure these variables, so we are limited in the extent to which we can interpret their findings (eg, it is unclear whether their measure of perceived stress pertained to stress in general or pandemic-related stress specifically).

The other four studies of internal resources yielded mixed results. In a sample of over 2700 adults residing in Italy during the 2020 lockdown, Barni et alCitation69 found that participants who knew someone that had contracted the coronavirus reported lower psychological wellbeing, but less so if they had a greater sense of coherence (SOC, ie, the view that one’s life and world are comprehensible, manageable, and meaningful). Participants with a greater fear of contracting the virus also reported lower wellbeing, but this association was stronger among those with greater SOC. However, this latter finding may indicate a floor effect, because those with greater SOC had higher wellbeing across all levels of fear. Faul and De BrigardCitation71 examined whether trait nostalgia served as a resilience resource for US adults in the first wave (study 1, April 2020) and second wave (study 2, August 2020) of the pandemic. In study 1 (involving 134 participants), those who reported greater concern about the pandemic were more likely to report worsened mood over the past month, but not if they were high in nostalgia. However, this moderation effect was not found for ratings of mood change over the past week. Furthermore, in study 2 (involving 159 participants), nostalgia did not moderate the association between pandemic concern and ratings of mood change over the past month nor the past week. In addition, in study 1, it was found that pandemic concern had a stronger negative association with positive mood among those lower in trait nostalgia, but again, this moderation effect was not replicated in study 2. During the April 2021 period, in a nationally representative daily-diary study of 444 US adolescents, Wang et alCitation76 found no evidence that either primary engagement coping (similar to problem-solving coping) or secondary engagement coping (similar to emotion regulation) moderated the negative association between daily health-related stress (including pandemic-related stress) and either same-day or next-day positive affect. Primary engagement coping also had no moderation effects on negative affect, although secondary engagement coping mitigated the negative association between health-related stress and both same-day and next-day negative affect. Wang et al also measured whether an external resource – parental support – moderated the relationship between daily stress and positive or negative affect. Parental support did not moderate the relationship with positive affect, and although it significantly moderated the relationship with negative affect, the data showed that parental support was not a significant moderator at higher levels of stress.

Three other studies investigated moderators other than internal resources. In a longitudinal study of the mental health outcomes of over 5000 Israeli children and adolescents in the period from pre-pandemic (September 2019) to post-fifth wave (May 2022), ShoshaniCitation75 observed smaller reductions in positive emotion, life satisfaction, and gratitude among participants who reported higher levels of social support and/or higher levels of daily routine (eg, more frequent engagement in social activities, family rituals, distance learning tasks). In a study of over 900 US adults during the first wave of the pandemic (March-April, 2020), Cunningham et alCitation70 found that those who reported greater social isolation had lower levels of positive affect. This negative association was stronger among older adults, possibly indicating a floor effect, because older age was associated with greater positive affect at all levels of social isolation. Indeed, the positive associations between social isolation and negative outcomes (including negative affect and depressive symptoms) were weaker among older adults. In a study of over 800 Iranian adults, Haghayeghi et alCitation72 found that participants who rated the pandemic as more disruptive to their lives reported lower levels of psychological wellbeing, but this negative association was weaker among those who reported smaller pandemic-related reductions in physical activity.

In summary, a range of resilience resources have been investigated as moderators of the effects of COVID-19 on positive affect, including internal resources such as trait resilience and other factors such as physical activity and age. Only two studies examined external resilience resources (parental support, social support, daily routine) in this context, with mixed findings.

Positive Affect as a Moderating Resilience Resource

As mentioned earlier, most of the studies retrieved from our literature search were concerned with positive affect as an outcome of the resilience process, rather than as a resilience resource. However, of the few studies that examined positive affect as a moderator, there were at least two that fell under each of the six subheadings employed in the previous section of this review (except COVID-19, for which there was only one study), so we have retained these subheadings in the present section. See for a summary of the key variables in these studies.

Table 2 Summary of Reviewed Studies on Positive Affect as a Resilience Resource

Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACEs)

In contrast to the relatively large number of studies examining the effects of ACEs on positive affect as an outcome, when we considered positive affect as a resilience resource, only two studies fit our search criteria. Both of these studies involved adult populations with negative childhood experiences that were retrospectively reported. In one study, involving a representative sample of over 19,000 US adults residing in Washington State, Logan-Greene et alCitation78 found that recalled ACEs were associated with poor physical and mental health among those with low life satisfaction, but not among those with high satisfaction. In the other study, of 381 Turkish university students, ArslanCitation77 measured the relationship between recalled ACEs (in the form of psychological maltreatment by parents) and social wellbeing, with trait resilience as a mediator and positive affect as a moderator. The author found a stronger negative association between recalled maltreatment and social wellbeing among those with lower positive affect. Interestingly, Arslan also found a stronger negative association between maltreatment and trait resilience among those with higher positive affect, but this might indicate a floor effect, because those with higher positive affect reported greater trait resilience at all levels of maltreatment.

Adult Experiences of Mistreatment or Trauma

Our search identified only two studies of positive affect as a resilience resource in the context of adult experiences of mistreatment or trauma. Consistent with the ACE studies that suggested a buffering role of positive affect, both studies on adult trauma found significant effects of positive affect-related moderators. In a sample of 81 Aboriginal Australian community adults attending a family counselling service, Gee et alCitation79 found a moderating effect of resilience resources encompassing both personal and relational/cultural strengths (and including a measure of positive affect). Those who had experienced more potentially traumatic events in their lifetimes reported higher levels of posttraumatic stress symptoms, but this association was not significant among those with high levels of strengths. In a study of 425 US female undergraduates who had previously experienced sexual assault, Kumar et alCitation80 examined the potential buffering roles of optimism and gratitude (both involving positive affect) in the relationship between posttraumatic stress and suicide risk. Both optimism and gratitude acted as protective factors, where higher levels of either resource predicted a weaker association between posttraumatic stress symptoms and suicidal ideation. This is consistent with the idea that both optimism and gratitude function to pivot one’s focus toward favourable aspects of life, thereby promoting mental health.

Stressful Life Events or Everyday Stressors

Five studies (across four papers) have examined positive affect as a resilience resource in the context of stressful life events. In a longitudinal study of 126 German university students, Brailovskaia et alCitation81 found that stressful life events at baseline predicted greater suicidal ideation both at baseline and 24 months later, but these associations were far weaker among those with higher baseline levels of mental wellbeing (including positive affect and other aspects of wellbeing). Similarly, in a cross-sectional study of over 1300 US alcohol-dependent patients, Kathryn McHugh et alCitation83 observed a smaller positive association between perceived stress and negative affect among those with higher positive affect (however, although positive affect was negatively correlated with alcohol cravings, it did not moderate the positive association between perceived stress and cravings). Together, these results suggest that positive affect may serve as a resilience resource even in the context of more severe outcomes such as suicidality and more severe stressors such as substance addiction.

However, other research has produced less consistent findings. Riskind et alCitation84 conducted two studies of resilience resources in relation to positive and negative affect, positive and negative life events, and depression and anxiety symptoms. In the first study, of 99 US undergraduate students, baseline trait negative affect positively predicted depressive symptoms six weeks later in those with low baseline trait positive affect, but not in those with high baseline trait positive affect. However, trait positive affect did not have such an interaction effect on anxiety symptoms. In the second study, of 107 US undergraduates, there was an interaction between number of positive events (in the past year) and number of negative events (in the past year) in predicting depression symptoms (but again, not anxiety) six weeks later. For those who had experienced a low number of positive events, there was a positive association between number of negative events and depression. However, for those who had experienced a high number of positive events, there was a negative association between negative events and depression. Thus, experiencing fewer negative events was seemingly a liability for those who reported a high number of positive events. This counter-intuitive finding warrants further exploration.

Finally, a single study has considered genetic factors in the context of positive affect as a potential moderator of the impact of stressful life events. In a sample of over 13,000 members of the Netherlands Twin Register (including pairs of twins and their biological siblings), de Vries et alCitation82 investigated the longitudinal association (over periods of up to 10 years) between wellbeing and resilience to negative life events. Resilience was operationalised as the residuals from regressing a measure of anxiety and depression symptoms on a measure of the number of negative events reported by the participants in their lifetimes. Thus, higher resilience scores indicated lower levels of anxiety and depression than would have been expected given the number of reported negative events. After accounting for genetic overlap, the authors found evidence of a causal effect of life satisfaction on resilience, with those higher in life satisfaction reporting lower levels of anxiety and depression for a given number of negative events. However, there was also evidence of a causal effect in the opposite direction, whereby resilience positively predicted wellbeing at follow-up.

Occupational Stressors

Two cross-sectional studies, both conducted in Italy, have examined positive affect as a moderator of the impact of occupational stressors. In one study, of 222 patient-facing healthcare professionals, Maffoni et alCitation85 found that managerial support (in dealing with ethical issues at work) was negatively correlated with moral distress (and thereby emotional exhaustion) only in those with higher levels of positive affect. This finding suggests that positive affect accentuates the benefits of managerial support, but conversely, it can be interpreted as suggesting that positive affect worsens the distress caused by a lack of managerial support. Clearly, this domain warrants further research. In the other study, of 131 never-employed young adults, Viola et alCitation86 found that psychological wellbeing was negatively associated with lack of career readiness (ie, career indecision and lack of motivation to find employment) more strongly in those with lower levels of hardiness (ie, lower trait resilience). This interaction effect suggests that wellbeing might be a more impactful resource for those with lower trait resilience, at least in some contexts. However, it could also be interpreted in the reverse direction, with lack of career readiness as the stressor, wellbeing as the outcome, and hardiness as the resilience resource. Again, further research is required.

Health Conditions (Other Than COVID-19)

Four studies have examined positive affect as a possible protective factor for those with various health conditions. Two of these studies found that positive affect (or related wellbeing measures) significantly moderated the association between depression and suicidality. In a longitudinal study of over 1900 US adolescents, Yu et alCitation89 classified the participants into three profiles: low levels, mild levels, and moderate-to-high levels of depressive symptoms. Regardless of life satisfaction, those in the low profile had a low risk of suicide, and those in the moderate-to-high profile had a high risk of suicide. But in the mild profile, life satisfaction was negatively associated with suicide risk. This suggests that while life satisfaction may serve to mitigate the impact of mild depression, it may not be so effective in the context of more severe depression. However, Yu et al also found that there was a negative association between optimism and suicide risk only for the moderate-to-high profile, which suggests that positive affect-related traits (such as optimism) may serve as resilience resources even for those with severe depression. Furthermore, Siegmann et alCitation87 examined whether moderators of the association between depression and suicidal ideation were the same cross-culturally in large samples of German (601 participants) and Chinese (over 2600 participants) university students. In both populations, the negative association between depression severity and suicidal ideation was weaker for participants who reported higher levels of mental wellbeing (including positive emotions). Life satisfaction played a similar buffering role, but only for the German participants.

In addition, two studies have found evidence of the possible protective role of positive affect for adults experiencing chronic physical pain. In one study, of 43 rheumatoid arthritis (RA) patients, Strand et alCitation88 made weekly measurements of the severity of the participants’ most intense RA-related pain. Those with more severe pain reported higher levels of negative affect, but this association was weaker among those with higher positive affect. In the other study, of 124 women with fibromyalgia and/or osteoarthritis, Zautra et alCitation90 measured the participants’ average weekly levels of both physical pain (due to their health condition) and perceived interpersonal stress. Both pain and interpersonal stress were positively associated with negative affect, but less so among those with higher positive affect. Together, these studies suggest that positive affect can be an effective resilience resource even in the context of severe physical pain.

COVID-19

A single study investigated positive affect as a resilience resource in the context of the COVID-19 pandemic. In a longitudinal study of 292 US university students, Venkatesh et alCitation91 administered five surveys in the period May-December 2020. Higher levels of pandemic-related stress at a given timepoint predicted higher levels of depression symptoms at the next timepoint, but this positive association was weaker among those with greater positive affect at the earlier timepoint. However, this moderation effect was not found when anxiety, physical symptoms, or overall health were the outcomes. Furthermore, although the stress-depression link was stronger among those with lower positive affect, those with higher positive affect tended to report greater depression across almost all levels of stress, suggesting that positive affect was mostly a liability rather than a buffer. Despite this, the authors interpreted the interaction effect as if it revealed a protective effect. At most, this conclusion was warranted only at the highest levels of pandemic-related stress.

Discussion

Summary of Review

In the context of resilience, mental health research has examined positive affect either as an outcome of the resilience process or as a resource that enables or promotes this process. In both roles, positive affect has been studied in relation to six broad categories of stressor: ACEs, PTEs in adulthood, general life stress, occupational stress, health-related stressors (except COVID-19), and COVID-19-related stress.

Studies of positive affect as an outcome have suggested that the negative impact of these various stressors can be mitigated by a diverse range of resilience resources. These include internal resources such as personality traits and other personal attributes, as well as external resources such as interpersonal connections and social factors. Numerous studies (eg,Citation19,Citation28,Citation36,Citation58,Citation68,Citation74) have found that those with higher trait resilience experience smaller reductions (or larger rebounds) in positive affect following a stressor. The possible protective roles of several other traits have also been revealed. These traits typically involve a positive attitude toward the future (eg, hope,Citation38 optimism,Citation53,Citation64 positive expectancy,Citation30 reward sensitivityCitation34), a positive attitude toward oneself (eg, self-efficacy,Citation64 self-esteem,Citation53,Citation64 self-compassion,Citation33 internal locus of control,Citation31 perceived control,Citation24,Citation53 problem-solving coping style,Citation17 sense of ethnic identityCitation25), or the tendency to see positives or meaning in one’s life (eg, trait benefit-finding,Citation61 sense of coherence,Citation69 meaning-focused coping style,Citation54 sense of life meaning,Citation18 belief in a just worldCitation27). The Big Five personality traits have also been examined as resilience resources, with evidence that Neuroticism exacerbates the negative impact of stressors on positive affect, while the other four traits (especially Conscientiousness and Extraversion) mitigate this impact.Citation32,Citation47,Citation59 A few studiesCitation41,Citation44,Citation48 have also examined the possible protective role of emotion regulation, finding that those who employ cognitive reappraisal are less negatively affected by adversity.

When we consider the psychological processes by which each of the aforementioned traits may enable resilience, the most plausible mechanisms are usually discernible from the name of the trait itself (or the category of traits to which it belongs). For example, we may readily hypothesise that the future-oriented traitsCitation38,Citation53,Citation64 promote resilience via future-directed thoughts and behaviours. One may be more motivated to overcome adversity if one can envision a positive future awaiting oneself (making the present hardship worth one’s while), and one’s responses to adversity may be more effective if one is better able to conceive of viable paths linking present actions with future outcomes (see Snyder’s Hope TheoryCitation92). However, when we consider trait resilience specifically, the relevant psychological mechanisms are not immediately evident. This is because the term “trait resilience” is used in disparate ways across the literature. For example, one of the most popular trait measures of resilience – the Connor-Davidson Resilience Scale (CD-RISC)Citation93 – comprises five dimensions, covering a wide range of characteristics such as personal competence, acceptance of change, relationship security, perceived control, and spiritual attitudes. In contrast, another popular trait measure – the Brief Resilience Scale (BRS)Citation94 – does not refer to any personal resources at all. Instead, the BRS simply asks the respondent whether or not they tend to recover quickly and easily from hardship (via six essentially synonymous questions). Thus, scales such as the CD-RISC actually measure one or more traits (eg, perceived control) that may serve as resilience resources, whereas scales such as the BRS pose autobiographical questions about the extent to which the respondent typically recovers from adversity, without inquiring about the personal attributes that may promote or impede such recovery. Therefore, we advise the reader that any findings pertaining to trait resilience (in the present review and elsewhere) must be interpreted with reference to the specific measures used. Where multidimensional measures such as the CD-RISC have been used, the reader must speculate as to which of the incorporated traits might have been responsible for any observed resilience (unless sub-analyses of the individual scale dimensions were performed). Where autobiographical measures such as the BRS have been used, the reader is left to wonder whether any differences in resilience were due to genuine differences in (unmeasured) personality traits or instead due to non-personality differences that covary with self-reported recovery from adversity.

Furthermore, even when a unidimensional trait measure has been used (such as the 10-item version of the CD-RISCCitation95), the reader is not able to discern any plausible psychological mechanisms without inspecting the individual items comprising the scale. Upon doing so, the reader may discover that many of the items are autobiographical rather than personality-related (at least at face value). For example, in the 10-item CD-RISC, some of the items arguably refer to traits such as self-efficacy (eg, “I see myself as a strong person”) or a sense of humour (eg, “I try to see the humorous side of problems”), but many of them refer only to the phenomenon of resilience itself (eg, “I tend to bounce back after illness or hardship”, “I can achieve goals despite obstacles”, “I am not easily discouraged by failure”). Such items may serve as useful measures of resilience insofar as they correlate strongly with the phenomenon, but they do not shed any light on how the process of resilience unfolds nor the psychological mechanisms that enable it. Indeed, to answer the question of why a given individual tends to bounce back from adversity, it would be both pointless and circular to mention that the individual strongly agrees with the statement “I tend to bounce back from adversity”. In short, we caution the reader to be mindful of circular reasoning (ie, “He’s resilient because he’s high in trait resilience”) and to examine the specific scales and items subsumed under the label “trait resilience” in order to interpret the relevant studies accurately.

Shifting from personality characteristics to interpersonal or social factors, a range of resilience resources have been found to be potentially protective of positive affect. These include social support (given and received),Citation56 having a romantic partner,Citation62 teacher support (for students),Citation47 positive peer relations,Citation40 parent-child communication,Citation47 sense of community,Citation15 family meals,Citation43 outdoor excursions,Citation46 volunteering,Citation66 and cross-ethnic friendships.Citation9 Other relevant moderators include personal characteristics that relate to social factors, such as loneliness (exacerbating stress)Citation32 and social support-focused coping style (mitigating stress).Citation54

As the preceding summary demonstrates, studies of positive affect as a resilience outcome have focused primarily on resilience resources of a personal or interpersonal nature, especially personality traits and various forms of social support or social connection. Aside from these resources, we note that only a single study has examined each of the following moderators of the link between adversity and positive affect: physical exercise,Citation14 educational attainment,Citation32 daily routines,Citation75 vagal suppression,Citation10 and low heart rate reactivity.Citation20 In addition, a small number of studies have examined whether a given form of adversity can moderate its own impact (or the impact of other forms of adversity) on positive affect, with mixed findings. There is evidence that the negative effects of one stressor can be compounded by those of another stressor,Citation15,Citation23,Citation37 but there is also evidence of curvilinear relationships whereby a low or moderate level of adversity (either concurrently or in one’s past) can be protective of positive affect, relative to no adversity or high levels of adversity.Citation35,Citation45,Citation49 The latter findings concur with the literature on stress inoculation and post-traumatic growth,Citation96,Citation97 reminding us of the formative role that hardship can play in the development of mental health and fortitude.

Although dozens of studies have examined positive affect as an outcome of the resilience process, far fewer studies have treated positive affect as a resilience resource that can mitigate the impact of stressors on other outcomes. Moreover, the latter studies pertain to a relatively narrow range of mental health outcomes, predominantly comprising symptoms related to depressionCitation82,Citation84 or suicide,Citation80,Citation81,Citation87,Citation89 or other negative outcomes such as anxiety, trauma, or negative affect.Citation82,Citation83,Citation88,Citation90 Other outcomes, including moral distress,Citation85 career readiness,Citation86 social wellbeing,Citation77 and physical health,Citation78 have each been addressed by a single study only.

Despite the relative lack of studies on positive affect as a resilience resource, a number of key findings are evident from the literature. In the domain of ACEs, positive affect may mitigate the impact of early life stress on both physical and mental health outcomes.Citation78 Regarding PTEs in adulthood, positive affect may reduce the risks of posttraumatic stress and suicidality.Citation79,Citation80 Similarly, in the context of stressful life events, positive affect may be protective against negative affect, depression, anxiety, and suicidal ideation.Citation81–84 Indeed, when depression itself is the stressor, positive affect may be protective against suicidality.Citation87,Citation89 Although these key findings are promising, far more research is needed in order to understand the full scope of positive affect as a resilience resource.

Theoretical Implications

A key theoretical implication of our review is that it reinforces the conception of mental and emotional health as comprising two overlapping but distinct areas of functioning, pertaining to positive emotions and mental wellbeing on the one hand versus negative emotions and mental illness on the other. Where appropriate, we have noted throughout the review those studies which yielded divergent results with regard to positive versus negative constructs (eg, a resilience resource that mitigated a stressor’s impact on positive but not negative affect). Such findings corroborate our modern understanding of mental health, often termed the dual-continua model.Citation98

Another implication pertains to the nature of moderation effects and how they are interpreted. Without a graph of an interaction effect (or inspection of the corresponding conditional means), the effect cannot be interpreted merely from the associated regression coefficient. For example, a positive coefficient might reveal that the negative association between a stressor and positive affect becomes less negative at higher levels of the resilience resource in question. However, while this seems to indicate that the resource is protective, such a coefficient could instead originate from a floor or ceiling effect. For instance, those with high levels of the supposedly protective resource might have low levels of positive affect across all values of the stressor, in which case the resource would actually confer no advantage at high levels of stress and a disadvantage at low levels of stress. For most of the studies in our review, the authors provided sufficient graphical or numerical summaries for us to determine the exact nature of the interaction effects tested. Where appropriate, we have offered alternative interpretations of results whose authors may have omitted the necessary data or neglected to consider all explanations of their findings. Overall, we recommend that future scientists not only perform moderation analyses of candidate resilience resources (rather than testing only direct effects or mediations) but also publish all information needed to interpret the analyses accurately (ideally in graphical form).

We raise the issue of moderation analyses not only as a methodological critique but also to highlight some of the interesting findings of our review. In almost every study that yielded a floor or ceiling effect, the purported resilience resource conferred little or no advantage at high levels of adversity and a large advantage at low levels of adversity. In these cases, while there is no evidence that the resource is protective against the stressor in question, the resource can still be considered a net asset, being associated with positive outcomes at least at low levels of the stressor. Such results invite researchers to speculate on the possible causes of the ceiling or floor effect. It may be that the stressor is especially impactful, reducing everyone’s wellbeing to a minimal level (at high levels of the stressor) regardless of individual differences in the purported resilience resource. Or it may be that the resource actively compounds the benefits that exist at low levels of adversity (eg, it might accentuate the mental health benefits of relaxation or contentment) while being inert at high levels of adversity. These theoretical questions warrant exploration in future research.

Gaps and Future Directions

Our literature review has encompassed various studies covering a wide range of stressors, resilience resources, and outcomes. These studies reveal an evidence base with many gaps, but each gap represents not only missing knowledge but also opportunities for future research.

One major gap is evident from the body of this review: Relatively few studies have treated positive affect as a resilience resource. As discussed earlier, it is understandable that many researchers would treat affective states as outcomes of antecedent experiences, but it is also important to remember the motivational role of these states. From an evolutionary perspective, it is obvious that affective states serve to motivate productive behaviours in countless settings, so it is incumbent on resilience researchers not to neglect the possible protective role of positive emotional responses in the context of stress or adversity.

Another major gap pertains to the ways in which positive affect has been measured in the resilience context. As shown in , many resilience studies have employed outcome measures that incorporate positive affect but do not measure it as a standalone variable. If we had restricted our analysis only to those studies with a “pure” measure of positive affect, our review would have covered far fewer studies and areas of interest. Future scientists may wish to employ specific measures of positive affect in replications of those studies which did not include such measures originally, to evaluate whether the effect still holds for positive affect when taking into consideration related constructs.

Our review has covered numerous stressors, resources, and outcomes, as well as a wide range of countries, populations, and settings, but this diversity is both a strength and a limitation. Obviously, it is desirable to study positive affect and resilience across varying contexts, but the variability of the literature is also a measure of the lack of replications therein. Direct replications are missing from the literature, and few studies have examined the same populations, stressors, moderators, or outcomes in the same ways. Even where, for example, a resilience resource (such as trait resilience) has been investigated by multiple studies, the studies employ differing measures of the resource or differing measures of the associated outcomes. Again, this is not merely a limitation but also a substantial opening for future inquiry. In addition, there have been relatively few studies that employed experimental or clinical trial designs in the area of positive affect and resilience. This is understandable, given the ethical constraints involved in research on stress and trauma, but there is still scope for innovative experiments and interventions to test resilience resources in more rigorous and realistic ways. Another gap relates to the kinds of stressors examined in the literature to date. Understandably, much of the research has focused on more severe or salient stressors (such as child maltreatment), but we must remember that resilience pertains to not only recovery from adverse events but also adaptation to stressors more broadly, including life challenges that may be demanding but fulfilling (eg, competitive sport). Future resilience researchers may wish to explore the roles of positive affect in the context of such stressors.

A key strength of our review is that it employed a rigorous definition of resilience, whereby resilience resources moderate the impact of a given stressor on a given mental health outcome. We also included all null results of the moderation analyses reported in the included studies, to give the most complete portrayal of the literature possible. However, we must stress that while a given variable may not have an interactive effect within the resilience process, it may still have an additive effect on the outcome in question. Thus, a given attribute might not serve as a resilience resource (in that it does not mitigate the impact of any stressor) but it might still be worth promoting such attributes because of other benefits they may bring beyond the context of recovery or adaptation. Furthermore, even when a significant interaction effect is present, the reader must exercise caution in interpreting the nature of the effect. We recommend consulting graphical depictions of such interactions, in order to assess whether any ceiling or floor effects may be influencing the interpretation (see our earlier discussion in Theoretical Implications).

Finally, we should note that some authors may be studying the resilience process without always using the terminology of resilience. To take a deliberately extreme example, if an author had studied recovery from adversity, but referred to this process exclusively with the term “amelioration”, then their study would not have been included in our review. This is not a weakness of the review because it would have been beyond the scope of this paper to include all synonyms of resilience. We simply wish to alert the reader to the possibility of alternative conceptualisations, and to recommend that future researchers use the terminology of resilience when studying the process of overcoming adversity or adapting to stressors, in order for their findings not to be overlooked by the mainstream of the field.

Concluding Remarks

The field of research on resilience and positive affect is relatively young, but the literature has already provided a wide array of studies covering numerous forms of stress and adversity. Most of the studies measured positive affect as an outcome of the resilience process, highlighting various internal and external resources that may be protective of positive affect and mental wellbeing in the face of stressors. Although relatively few studies have treated positive affect as a potential resilience resource, their findings show that positive affect may be protective of mental health following adversity in childhood, adulthood, and everyday life. In all, there are countless opportunities to build upon the knowledge base covered in this review. Numerous stressors, moderators, and outcomes have yet to be examined in the context of positive affect and resilience. Indeed, an inspection of the years of publication of the reviewed papers shows how many were published only in the last few years. Clearly, this field is expanding quickly, which aligns with the broader acceleration of modern psychological research on the positive dimensions of mental health. The potential for new discoveries, and thereby new approaches to helping people recover from or adapt to stress and trauma, seems immense.

Disclosure

LAE, HRPP, and JMG were supported by a National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC) Project Grant (1122816) awarded to JMG. JL was supported by a University of New South Wales Research Training Program PhD scholarship and a Neuroscience Research Australia PhD Pearl Program award. All authors declare that they have no other conflicts of interest in this work.

Unknown widget #5d0ef076-e0a7-421c-8315-2b007028953f

of type scholix-links

References

  • Keyes CLM. Mental illness and/or mental health? Investigating axioms of the complete state model of health. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2005;73(3):539–548. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.73.3.539
  • Keyes CLM. The mental health continuum: from languishing to flourishing in life. J Health Soc Behav. 2002;43(2):207–222. doi:10.2307/3090197
  • Maddux JE. Positive psychology and the illness ideology: toward a positive clinical psychology. Appl Psychol. 2008;57(SUPPL. 1):54–70. doi:10.1111/j.1464-0597.2008.00354.x
  • Seligman MEP, Steen TA, Park N, Peterson C. Positive psychology progress: empirical validation of interventions. Am Psychol. 2005;60(5):410–421. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.60.5.410
  • Alexander R, Gatt JM. Resilience. In: Miu AC, Homberg JR, Lesch K-P, editors. Genes, Brain and Emotions: Interdisciplinary and Translational Perspectives. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2019:286–303.
  • Layne CM, Beck CJ, Rimmasch H, Southwick JS, Moreno MA, Hobfoll SE. Promoting “Resilient” Posttraumatic Adjustment in Childhood and Beyond. Routledge; 2008. doi:10.4324/9780203893104
  • Chmitorz A, Kunzler A, Helmreich I, et al. Intervention studies to foster resilience – a systematic review and proposal for a resilience framework in future intervention studies. Clin Psychol Rev. 2018;59:78–100. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2017.11.002
  • Armitage JM, Wang RAH, Davis OSP, Haworth CMA. A polygenic approach to understanding resilience to peer victimisation. Behav Genet. 2022;52(1):1–12. doi:10.1007/s10519-021-10085-5
  • Bagci SC, Rutland A, Kumashiro M, Smith PK, Blumberg H. Are minority status children’s cross-ethnic friendships beneficial in a multiethnic context? Br J Dev Psychol. 2014;32(1):107–115. doi:10.1111/bjdp.12028
  • Cipriano EA, Skowron EA, Gatzke-Kopp LM. Preschool children’s cardiac reactivity moderates relations between exposure to family violence and emotional adjustment. Child Maltreat. 2011;16(3):205–215. doi:10.1177/1077559511408887
  • Cohen JR, Choi JW, Thakur H, Temple JR. Psychological distress and well-being in trauma-exposed adolescents: a residualized, person-centered approach to resilience. J Trauma Stress. 2021;34(3):487–500. doi:10.1002/jts.22646
  • DiClemente CM, Rice CM, Quimby D, et al. Resilience in urban African American adolescents: the protective enhancing effects of neighborhood, family, and school cohesion following violence exposure. J Early Adolesc. 2018;38(9):1286–1321. doi:10.1177/0272431616675974
  • Kang H, Rigazio-DiGilio SA, Super CM, Halgunseth LC. Resilience and well-being of Korean unwed mothers: a moderated mediation model. J Child Fam Stud. 2023;32(5):1332–1343. doi:10.1007/s10826-022-02509-5
  • Nishimi KM, Koenen KC, Coull BA, Kubzansky LD. Association of psychological resilience with healthy lifestyle and body weight in young adulthood. J Adolesc Heal. 2022;70(2):258–266. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2021.08.006
  • Nurius PS, Green S, Logan-Greene P, Borja S. Life course pathways of adverse childhood experiences toward adult psychological well-being: a stress process analysis. Child Abus Negl. 2015;45:143–153. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2015.03.008
  • Piña-Watson B, Llamas JD, Stevens AK. Attempting to successfully straddle the cultural divide: hopelessness model of bicultural stress, mental health, and caregiver connection for Mexican descent adolescents. J Couns Psychol. 2015;62(4):670–681. doi:10.1037/cou0000116
  • Riley F, Wright M, Bokszczanin A, Essau CA. Poly-Victimization in polish adolescents: risk factors and the moderating role of coping. J Interpers Violence. 2020;35(5–6):1515–1538. doi:10.1177/0886260517696868
  • Seon Y, Smith-Adcock S. Adolescents’ meaning in life as a resilience factor between bullying victimization and life satisfaction. Child Youth Serv Rev. 2023;148:106875. doi:10.1016/j.childyouth.2023.106875
  • Sleijpen M, van der Aa N, Mooren T, Laban CJ, Kleber RJ. The moderating role of individual resilience in refugee and dutch adolescents after trauma. Psychol Trauma. 2019;11(7):732–742. doi:10.1037/tra0000450
  • Somers JA, Ibrahim MH, Luecken LJ. Biological sensitivity to the effects of childhood family adversity on psychological well-being in young adulthood. Child Maltreat. 2017;22(3):236–244. doi:10.1177/1077559517711041
  • Sorek Y. Grandparental and overall social support as resilience factors in coping with parental conflict among children of divorce. Child Youth Serv Rev. 2020;118:105443. doi:10.1016/j.childyouth.2020.105443
  • Sorek Y. Children of divorce evaluate their quality of life: the moderating effect of psychological processes. Child Youth Serv Rev. 2019;107(October). doi:10.1016/j.childyouth.2019.104533
  • Tung NYC, Yap Y, Bei B, Luecken LJ, Wiley JF. Resilience to early family risk moderates stress-affect associations: a 14-day ecological momentary assessment study. J Affect Disord. 2022;311:134–142. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2022.05.075
  • Turiano NA, Silva NM, McDonald C, Hill PL. Retrospective reports of childhood misfortune are associated with positive and negative affect in adulthood: exploring the moderating role of control beliefs. Int J Aging Hum Dev. 2017;84(3):276–293. doi:10.1177/0091415016688480
  • Fetter AK, Thompson MN. The impact of historical loss on Native American college students’ mental health: the protective role of ethnic identity. J Couns Psychol. 2023. doi:10.1037/cou0000686
  • Lee H, Aldwin CM, Kang S, Ku X. Vulnerability and resilience among older south Korean Vietnam war veterans: a latent profile analysis. Psychol Serv. 2021;19(2011):95–105. doi:10.1037/ser0000568
  • Liu X, Xie T, Li W, et al. The relationship between perceived discrimination and wellbeing in impoverished college students: a moderated mediation model of self-esteem and belief in a just world. Curr Psychol. 2023;42(8):6711–6721. doi:10.1007/s12144-021-01981-4
  • Yubero S, de Las Heras M, Navarro R, Larrañaga E. Relations among chronic bullying victimization, subjective well-being and resilience in university students: a preliminary study. Curr Psychol. 2023;42(2):855–866. doi:10.1007/s12144-021-01489-x
  • Alriksson-Schmidt AI, Wallander J, Biasini F. Quality of life and resilience in adolescents with a mobility disability. J Pediatr Psychol. 2007;32(3):370–379. doi:10.1093/jpepsy/jsl002
  • Arampatzi E, Burger M, Stavropoulos S, Tay L. The Role of Positive Expectations for Resilience to Adverse Events: Subjective Well-Being Before, During and After the Greek Bailout Referendum. Vol. 21. the Netherlands: Springer; 2020. doi:10.1007/s10902-019-00115-9
  • Awaworyi Churchill S, Smyth R. Locus of control and the mental health effects of local area crime. Soc Sci Med. 2022;301:114910. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2022.114910
  • Bucknor BA, Derringer J. Exploring the utility of current polygenic scores in capturing resilience. Psychiatr Genet. 2022;32(1):15–24. doi:10.1097/YPG.0000000000000300
  • Chong ESK, Mohr JJ, Chui H. Examining the protective role of self-compassion in the links between daily sexual orientation salient experiences and affect. J Couns Psychol. 2023. doi:10.1037/cou0000690
  • Corral-Frías NS, Nadel L, Fellous JM, Jacobs WJ. Behavioral and self-reported sensitivity to reward are linked to stress-related differences in positive affect. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2016;66:205–213. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.01.012
  • Dooley LN, Slavich GM, Moreno PI, Bower JE. Strength through adversity: moderate lifetime stress exposure is associated with psychological resilience in breast cancer survivors. Stress Heal. 2017;33(5):549–557. doi:10.1002/smi.2739
  • Ertl MM, Trapp SK, González Arredondo S, Rodríguez Agudelo Y, Arango-Lasprilla JC. Perceived stress, resilience, and health-related quality of life among Parkinson’s disease caregivers in Mexico. Heal Soc Care Community. 2019;27(5):1303–1310. doi:10.1111/hsc.12767
  • Gerstberger L, Blanke ES, Keller J, Brose A. Stress buffering after physical activity engagement: an experience sampling study. Br J Health Psychol. 2023;28(3):876–892. doi:10.1111/bjhp.12659
  • Goodman FR, Disabato DJ, Kashdan TB, Machell KA. Personality Strengths as Resilience: a One-Year Multiwave Study. J Pers. 2017;85(3):423–434. doi:10.1111/jopy.12250
  • Greenfield EA, Marks NF. Formal volunteering as a protective factor for older adults’ psychological well-being. J Gerontol Ser B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2004;59(5):258–264. doi:10.1093/geronb/59.5.S258
  • Hatzichristou C, Lianos P, Lampropoulou A, Stasinou V. Individual and systemic factors related to safety and relationships in schools as moderators of adolescents’ subjective well-being during unsettling times. Contemp Sch Psychol. 2020;24(3):252–265. doi:10.1007/s40688-020-00298-6
  • Hodzic S, Ripoll P, Costa H, Zenasni F. Are emotionally intelligent students more resilient to stress? The moderating effect of emotional attention, clarity and repair. Behav Psychol Psicol Conduct. 2016;24:253–272.
  • Huang S, Han M, Sun L, Zhang H, Li HJ. Family socioeconomic status and emotional adaptation among rural-to-urban migrant adolescents in China: the moderating roles of adolescent’s resilience and parental positive emotion. Int J Psychol. 2019;54(5):573–581. doi:10.1002/ijop.12499
  • Jagtiani MR, Kelly Y, Fancourt D, Shelton N, Scholes S. #StateOfMind: family meal frequency moderates the association between time on social networking sites and well-being among UK young adults. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Net. 2019;22(12):753–760. doi:10.1089/cyber.2019.0338
  • Johnson J, O’Connor DB, Jones C, Jackson C, Hughes GJ, Ferguson E. Reappraisal buffers the association between stress and negative mood measured over 14 days: implications for understanding psychological resilience. Eur J Pers. 2016;30(6):608–617. doi:10.1002/per.2080
  • Lazić M, Jovanović V, Gavrilov-Jerković V. The role of perceived negative childhood experiences in the context of recent stress: testing competing theoretical models. Curr Psychol. 2023;42(1):116–125. doi:10.1007/s12144-021-01418-y
  • Mutz M, Müller J, Göring A. Outdoor adventures and adolescents’ mental health: daily screen time as a moderator of changes. J Adventure Educ Outdoor Learn. 2019;19(1):56–66. doi:10.1080/14729679.2018.1507830
  • Noor NM, Alwi A. Stressors and well-being in low socio-economic status Malaysian adolescents: the role of resilience resources. Asian J Soc Psychol. 2013;16(4):292–306. doi:10.1111/ajsp.12035
  • Philippe FL, Dobbin AE, Ross S, Houle I. Resilience facilitates positive emotionality and integration of negative memories in need satisfying memory networks: an experimental study. J Posit Psychol. 2018;13(6):586–596. doi:10.1080/17439760.2017.1365158
  • Seery MD, Holman EA, Silver RC. Whatever does not kill us: cumulative lifetime adversity, vulnerability, and resilience. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2010;99(6):1025–1041. doi:10.1037/a0021344
  • Annor F, Amponsah-Tawiah K. Relationship between workplace bullying and employees’ subjective well-being: does resilience make a difference? Empl Responsib Rights J. 2020;32(3):123–135. doi:10.1007/s10672-020-09348-w
  • Bäccman C, Hjärthag F, Almqvist K. Improved resiliency and well-being among military personnel in a Swedish Naval Force after a counter-piracy operation off the coast of Somalia. Scand J Psychol. 2016;57(4):350–358. doi:10.1111/sjop.12299
  • Gabriel AS, Diefendorff JM, Erickson RJ. The relations of daily task accomplishment satisfaction with changes in affect: a multilevel study in nurses. J Appl Psychol. 2011;96(5):1095–1104. doi:10.1037/a0023937
  • Lai JC-L, Mak AJ-H. Resilience moderates the impact of daily hassles on positive well-being in Chinese undergraduates. J Psychol Chinese Soc. 2009;10(2):151–168.
  • Leipold B, Munz M, Michéle-Malkowsky A. Coping and resilience in the transition to adulthood. Emerg Adulthood. 2019;7(1):12–20. doi:10.1177/2167696817752950
  • Lutz S, Schneider FM, Vorderer P. On the downside of mobile communication: an experimental study about the influence of setting-inconsistent pressure on employees’ emotional well-being. Comput Human Behav. 2020;105:106216. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2019.106216
  • Sharda E. Parenting stress and well-being among foster parents: the moderating effect of social support. Child Adolesc Soc Work J. 2022;39(5):547–559. doi:10.1007/s10560-022-00836-6
  • Sharda EA, Sutherby CG, Cavanaugh DL, Hughes AK, Woodward AT. Parenting stress, well-being, and social support among kinship caregivers. Child Youth Serv Rev. 2019;99:74–80. doi:10.1016/j.childyouth.2019.01.025
  • Shi X, Gordon S, Tang CH. Momentary well-being matters: daily fluctuations in hotel employees’ turnover intention. Tour Manag. 2021;83(August 2020):104212. doi:10.1016/j.tourman.2020.104212
  • Udayar S, Urbanaviciute I, Massoudi K, Rossier J. The role of personality profiles in the longitudinal relationship between work-related well-being and life satisfaction among working adults in Switzerland. Eur J Pers. 2020;34(1):77–92. doi:10.1002/per.2225
  • van Erp KJPM, Rispens S, Gevers JMP, Demerouti E. When bystanders become bothersome: the negative consequences of bystander conflict and the moderating role of resilience. Eur J Work Organ Psychol. 2015;24(3):402–419. doi:10.1080/1359432X.2014.904290
  • Wepf H, Joseph S, Leu A. Benefit finding moderates the relationship between young carer experiences and mental well-being. Psychol Heal. 2022;37(10):1270–1286. doi:10.1080/08870446.2021.1941961
  • Bos EH, Snippe E, De Jonge P, Jeronimus BF. Preserving subjective wellbeing in the face of psychopathology: buffering effects of personal strengths and resources. PLoS One. 2016;11(3):1–14. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0150867
  • Delfabbro PH, Winefield AH, Anderson S, Hammarstrom A, Winefield H. Body image and psychological well-being in adolescents: the relationship between gender and school type. J Genet Psychol. 2011;172(1):67–83. doi:10.1080/00221325.2010.517812
  • Hajek A, König HH. The role of optimism, self-esteem, and self-efficacy in moderating the relation between health comparisons and subjective well-being: results of a nationally representative longitudinal study among older adults. Br J Health Psychol. 2019;24(3):547–570. doi:10.1111/bjhp.12367
  • Kratz AL, Davis MC, Zautra AJ. Pain acceptance moderates the relation between pain and negative affect in female osteoarthritis and fibromyalgia patients. Ann Behav Med. 2007;33(3):291–301. doi:10.1007/BF02879911
  • Okun M, Rios R, Crawford A, Levy R. Does the relation between volunteering and well-being vary with health and age? Int J Aging Hum Dev. 2011;72(3):265–287. doi:10.2190/AG.72.3.f
  • Plexico LW, Erath S, Shores H, Burrus E. Self-acceptance, resilience, coping and satisfaction of life in people who stutter. J Fluency Disord. 2019;59:52–63. doi:10.1016/j.jfludis.2018.10.004
  • Windle G, Woods RT, Markland DA. Living with ill-health in older age: the role of a resilient personality. J Happiness Stud. 2010;11(6):763–777. doi:10.1007/s10902-009-9172-3
  • Barni D, Danioni F, Canzi E, et al. Facing the COVID-19 Pandemic: the Role of Sense of Coherence. Front Psychol. 2020;11. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2020.578440
  • Cunningham TJ, Fields EC, Garcia SM, Kensinger EA. The relation between age and experienced stress, worry, affect, and depression during the spring 2020 Phase of the COVID-19 pandemic in the United States. Emotion. 2021;21(8):1660–1670. doi:10.1037/emo0000982
  • Faul L, De Brigard F. The moderating effects of nostalgia on mood and optimism during the COVID-19 pandemic. Memory. 2022;30(9):1103–1117. doi:10.1080/09658211.2022.2082481
  • Haghayeghi M, Mousavi A, Moghadamzadeh A, Naji SH. Reduction in physical activity moderates the effect of perceived disturbance from the COVID-19 pandemic on psychological well-being: a moderated-mediation approach. Humanist Psychol. 2023. doi:10.1037/hum0000329
  • Hu J, Ye B, Yildirim M, Yang Q. Perceived stress and life satisfaction during COVID-19 pandemic: the mediating role of social adaptation and the moderating role of emotional resilience. Psychol Heal Med. 2023;28(1):124–130. doi:10.1080/13548506.2022.2038385
  • Paredes MR, Apaolaza V, Fernandez-Robin C, Hartmann P, Yañez-Martinez D. The impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on subjective mental well-being: the interplay of perceived threat, future anxiety and resilience. Pers Individ Dif. 2021;170:110455. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2020.110455
  • Shoshani A. Longitudinal changes in children’s and adolescents’ mental health and well-being and associated protective factors during the COVID-19 pandemic. Psychol Trauma. 2023. doi:10.1037/tra0001556
  • Wang MT, Toro JD, Scanlon CL, et al. The roles of stress, coping, and parental support in adolescent psychological well-being in the context of COVID-19: a daily-diary study. J Affect Disord. 2021;294:245–253. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2021.06.082
  • Arslan G. Psychological maltreatment predicts decreases in social wellbeing through resilience in college students: a conditional process approach of positive emotions. Curr Psychol. 2023;42(3):2110–2120. doi:10.1007/s12144-021-01583-0
  • Logan-Greene P, Green S, Nurius PS, Longhi D. Distinct contributions of adverse childhood experiences and resilience resources: a cohort analysis of adult physical and mental health. Soc Work Health Care. 2014;53(8):776–797. doi:10.1080/00981389.2014.944251
  • Gee G, Hulbert C, Kennedy H, Paradies Y. Cultural determinants and resilience and recovery factors associated with trauma among Aboriginal help-seeking clients from an Aboriginal community-controlled counselling service. BMC Psychiatry. 2023;23(1):1–13. doi:10.1186/s12888-023-04567-5
  • Kumar SA, Jaffe AE, Brock RL, DiLillo D. Resilience to suicidal ideation among college sexual assault survivors: the protective role of optimism and gratitude in the context of posttraumatic stress. Psychol Trauma. 2021;14(S1):91–100. doi:10.1037/tra0001141
  • Brailovskaia J, Teismann T, Margraf J. Positive mental health, stressful life events, and suicide ideation: a 2-year follow-up study. Crisis. 2020;41(5):383–388. doi:10.1027/0227-5910/a000652
  • de Vries LP, Baselmans BML, Luykx JJ, et al. Genetic evidence for a large overlap and potential bidirectional causal effects between resilience and well-being. Neurobiol Stress. 2021;14(March). doi:10.1016/j.ynstr.2021.100315
  • Kathryn McHugh R, Kaufman JS, Frost KH, Fitzmaurice GM, Weiss RD. Positive affect and stress reactivity in alcohol-dependent outpatients. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2013;74(1):152–157. doi:10.15288/jsad.2013.74.152
  • Riskind JH, Kleiman EM, Schafer KE. Undoing” effects of positive affect: does it buffer the effects of negative affect in predicting changes in depression? J Soc Clin Psychol. 2013;32(4):363–380. doi:10.1521/jscp.2013.32.4.363
  • Maffoni M, Sommovigo V, Giardini A, Paolucci S, Setti I. Dealing with ethical issues in rehabilitation medicine: the relationship between managerial support and emotional exhaustion is mediated by moral distress and enhanced by positive affectivity and resilience. J Nurs Manag. 2020;28(5):1114–1125. doi:10.1111/jonm.13059
  • Viola MM, Musso P, Inguglia C, Lo Coco A. Psychological well-being and career indecision in emerging adulthood: the moderating role of hardiness. Career Dev Q. 2016;64(4):387–396. doi:10.1002/cdq.12073
  • Siegmann P, Teismann T, Fritsch N, et al. Resilience to suicide ideation: a cross-cultural test of the buffering hypothesis. Clin Psychol Psychother. 2018;25(1):e1–e9. doi:10.1002/cpp.2118
  • Strand EB, Zautra AJ, Thoresen M, Ødegård S, Uhlig T, Finset A. Positive affect as a factor of resilience in the pain-negative affect relationship in patients with rheumatoid arthritis. J Psychosom Res. 2006;60(5):477–484. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychores.2005.08.010
  • Yu J, Goldstein RB, Haynie DL, et al. Resilience factors in the association between depressive symptoms and suicidality. J Adolesc Heal. 2021;69(2):280–287. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2020.12.004
  • Zautra AJ, Johnson LM, Davis MC. Positive affect as a source of resilience for women in chronic pain. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2005;73(2):212–220. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.73.2.212
  • Venkatesh H, Osorno AM, Boehm JK, Jenkins BN. Resilience factors during the Coronavirus pandemic: testing the main effect and stress buffering models of optimism and positive affect with mental and physical health. J Health Psychol. 2023;28(5):405–416. doi:10.1177/13591053221120340
  • Snyder CR. TARGET ARTICLE: hope theory: rainbows in the mind. Psychol Inq. 2002;13(4):249–275. doi:10.1207/s15327965pli1304_01
  • Connor KM, Davidson JRT. Development of a new Resilience scale: the Connor-Davidson Resilience scale (CD-RISC). Depress Anxiety. 2003;18(2):76–82. doi:10.1002/da.10113
  • Smith BW, Dalen J, Wiggins K, Tooley E, Christopher P, Bernard J. The brief resilience scale: assessing the ability to bounce back. Int J Behav Med. 2008;15(3):194–200. doi:10.1080/10705500802222972
  • Campbell-Sills L, Stein MB. Psychometric analysis and refinement of theConnor–Davidson Resilience Scale (CD-RISC): validation of a 10-item measure of resilience. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 2007;20(6):1019–1028. doi:10.1002/jts.20271
  • Ord AS, Stranahan KR, Hurley RA, Taber KH. Stress-related growth: building a more resilient brain. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2020;32(3):206–212. doi:10.1176/appi.neuropsych.20050111
  • Woodward C, Joseph S. Positive change processes and post-traumatic growth in people who have experienced childhood abuse: understanding vehicles of change. Psychol Psychother Theory Res Pract. 2003;76(3):267–283. doi:10.1348/147608303322362497
  • Mason Stephens J, Iasiello M, Ali K, van Agteren J, Fassnacht DB. The importance of measuring mental wellbeing in the context of psychological distress: using a theoretical framework to test the dual-continua model of mental health. Behav Sci. 2023;13(5). doi:10.3390/bs13050436