1,175
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

What tools should be used to identify women in need of additional support in pregnancy?

ORCID Icon, & ORCID Icon
Pages 234-268 | Received 25 Apr 2022, Accepted 13 Jul 2022, Published online: 05 Aug 2022

ABSTRACT

Objective

This study aimed to identify and inform recommendation of self-report and interview-based instruments that are feasible, reliable and valid to evaluate the quality of the maternal-fetal relationship (MFR).

Background

Several constructs predicting parent–infant interaction and later infant adjustment are used to assess mothers’ thoughts and feelings towards their unborn baby, including reflective functioning, mind-mindedness, representation, and fetal attachment. As yet, there is no existing review comparing the quality and accessibility of instruments across each of these constructs.

Methods

A systematic literature review was undertaken to synthesise psychometric information on measures reporting on the MFR. Searches of six databases were conducted. English articles were selected based on inclusion and exclusion criteria. The QATSDD checklist was used to assess study quality.

Results

 Of 669 studies identified, 28 met inclusion criteria. Thirteen different instruments were identified for evaluation. Reported reliability and validity varied significantly across instruments, as well as availability for research and/or clinical use.

Conclusion

Suggestions for research and clinical practice include further evaluation of the psychometric properties of tools, particularly for self-report measures of reflective functioning, use of interviews to scaffold reflexivity, and development of clinical policies and procedures to clarify care pathways for expectant mothers needing further support.

Research into the maternal-fetal relationship (MFR) – expectant mothers’ thoughts and feelings about their unborn baby – began four decades ago with investigations into the quality of the maternal bond with the unborn baby – (‘maternal-fetal attachment’ ([MFA], Cranley, Citation1981)). Numerous other aspects of the maternal-fetal relationship have since been identified – ‘maternal-fetal representation’, ‘maternal reflective functioning’ and ‘maternal mind-mindedness’ – which focus at a more cognitive level on how the mother’s conscious and unconscious processes influence her thinking about her role as a mother and the relationship with her unborn baby.

The development of instruments measuring these four constructs has established their role as predictors of psychosocial risk factors (Lannert et al., Citation2013; Malone et al., Citation2010; Smaling et al., Citation2016), caregiving behaviours (Foley & Hughes, Citation2018) and later infant outcomes (Le Bas et al., Citation2020; Branjerdporn et al., Citation2017; Cataudella et al., Citation2016; Zeegers et al., Citation2017). Several reviews have reported on properties and associated outcomes of instruments assessing maternal-fetal attachment (e.g. Beck, Citation1999; Perrelli et al., Citation2014; Van den Bergh & Simons, Citation2009) and maternal representation, as measured by the Working Model of the Child Interview (e.g. Vreeswijk et al., Citation2012). Whilst recent literature has reviewed instruments measuring bonding in the perinatal period (Wittkowski et al., Citation2020), as yet no review has evaluated and compared instruments’ psychometric properties and usability across all four theoretical constructs linked to the MFR. This review aims to address this gap by summarising the psychometric properties of instruments used to measure these constructs and making recommendations regarding the suitability of each tool for use in clinical practice.

How are constructs relating to the maternal-fetal relationship defined?

Four constructs are proposed to describe aspects of the maternal-fetal relationship (). These include the way expectant mothers think about and bond with their unborn infant (e.g. maternal representation and maternal-fetal attachment), as well as parental mentalisation constructs (e.g. mind-mindedness and reflective functioning).

Table 1. Definitions associated with each of the four constructs.

Antenatal maternal representation describes the qualities with which a mother-to-be thinks about her unborn infant, herself as a mother and her experience of pregnancy based on her own internal representations (Rosenblum et al., Citation2009). These are templates, internalised from her own early relationships, that guide expectations and behaviours in later relationships (Bowlby, Citation1969). Maternal-fetal attachment (MFA) refers to the emotional bond the mother feels towards her unborn baby (Bowlby, Citation1982; Condon & Corkindale, Citation1997; Cranley, Citation1981). Condon (Citation1993) proposes a model for conceptualising the presence and quality of MFA, highlighting expectant-mothers’ desire to ‘get to know’, protect and take care of their unborn child.

Reflective functioning is the measure of an individual’s capacity to mentalise; that is, ‘to perceive and interpret human behaviours in terms of intentional mental states such as feelings, wishes, goals or desires’ (Fonagy et al., Citation1995, Citation1991). Reflective functioning has a particular bearing on parent–infant relationships, driving parents’ curiosity to understand, anticipate, empathise and respond to their child’s needs (Slade et al., Citation2005). Low levels of parental reflective functioning are demonstrated in an unawareness, inaccuracy or lack of comprehension of the internal state of the infant (Fonagy et al., Citation2016; Meins et al., Citation2002; Slade, Citation2005). Mind-mindedness (Meins, Citation1997, Citation1998) refers to the tendency for a caregiver to view their unborn baby as an individual with a capacity for a mind, ‘rather than merely as a creature with needs that must be satisfied’ (Meins, Fernyhough, Fradley et al., Citation2001, p. 638). In contrast to reflective functioning, which is assessed through invitations to share active reflections, mind-mindedness requires a spontaneous focus on mental states or attributes when describing their infant (McMahon & Bernier, Citation2017; Meins et al., Citation2012; Meins, Fernyhough, Russell et al., Citation2001).

Why assess the maternal-fetal relationship?

Pregnancy is a transitional period, during which the mother’s representations of herself and others, internalised from early experience, activate and organise expectations of her unborn child and herself as mother-to-be (e.g. Ammaniti et al., Citation1992; Cohen & Slade, Citation1999; Stern, Citation1995). Previous research demonstrates that the quality of the maternal-fetal relationship (MFR) predicts multiple infant outcomes, including perceptions about infant temperament, parent–infant interaction quality, infant attachment security, adaptive behaviour and milestone attainment (Le Bas et al., Citation2020; Branjerdporn et al., Citation2017; Crawford & Benoit, Citation2009; Foley & Hughes, Citation2018; Korja et al., Citation2010; Siddiqui & Hägglöf, Citation2000).

A recent meta-analysis (Foley & Hughes, Citation2018) investigating concordance between antenatal parental representations and later outcomes demonstrates a moderate association between antenatal relationships (including measures of parental representation and fetal attachment) and postnatal parent–infant interaction. Furthermore, research investigating antenatal reflective functioning has found positive associations with maternal sensitivity postnatally (Smaling et al., Citation2016). Similarly, parents demonstrating greater antenatal mind-mindedness are likely to comment appropriately and spontaneously on their infants’ internal states (Arnott & Meins, Citation2007) and show better quality parent–infant interactions (Arnott & Meins, Citation2007). Systematic review evidence from a range of postnatal timepoints demonstrates a positive relationship between mind-mindedness and child developmental outcomes (McMahon & Bernier, Citation2017).

Studies have also demonstrated that maternal mentalisation can predict infant–parent attachment quality, which in addition to parental sensitivity, has been suggested as a mechanism to explain the intergenerational transmission of attachment security (Oppenheim & Koren‐Karie, Citation2002; Slade, Grienenberger et al., Citation2005; Zeegers et al., Citation2017). A recent study on maternal representation found a correspondence between mothers’ classifications on the WMCI in pregnancy and Adult Attachment Interview (AAI). Follow-up mediation analyses showed that the WMCI accounted for the association between AAI and infant attachment security (Madigan et al., Citation2015). The fact that this process between mother and baby begins antenatally provides an important window of opportunity to reduce the risks within the parent–infant relationship.

What instruments measure the maternal-fetal relationship?

To date, studies investigating the MFR have used either self-report questionnaires to record expectant mothers’ thoughts and feelings about their unborn baby, or interview-based instruments, coded to draw out mothers’ representations and reflexive capacities. Recent research comparing interview-measures and questionnaire-based tools found no evidence to suggest greater predictive validity of either type of instrument for postnatal parent–infant interaction quality (Foley & Hughes, Citation2018). To the authors’ knowledge, this has not yet been investigated for mentalisation constructs.

Maternal representation

Standardised semi-structured interviews assess both cognitive and affective features of maternal representation through categorising narrative patterns (Zeanah, Citation2000). Examples of these include the Working Model of the Child Interview (WMCI; C.H. Zeanah et al., Citation1986), administered during or after pregnancy; the Interview of Maternal Representations During Pregnancy (IRMAG; Ammaniti et al., Citation1992; Ammaniti, Tambelli & Peucchini, Citation2000); the Pregnancy Interview (PI; Slade et al., Citation2003, 2005) and the Pregnancy Interview Revised (PI-R; Smaling et al., Citation2016). Self-report questionnaire methods can also be used to measure perinatal representations (e.g. Child Concept Questionnaire; Gloger-Tippelt, Citation1983).

Maternal-fetal attachment (MFA)

MFA, which has been most widely measured, is assessed using three different tools: (1) the Maternal-fetal Attachment Scale (MFAS; Cranley, Citation1981) primarily measures affiliative behaviours, (2) the Prenatal Attachment Inventory (PAI; Muller & Mercer, Citation1993) concentrates more on the emotional relationship to the fetus, whilst (3) the Maternal Antenatal Attachment Scale (MAAS; Condon, Citation1993) captures both the ‘quality’ (e.g. closeness/distance; tenderness/irritation; joyful/unpleasant) and the ‘intensity’ of the relationship to the unborn baby (e.g. talking to the baby; dreaming about or interacting with him/her).

Reflective functioning and mind-mindedness

Antenatal reflective functioning can be measured using semi-structured interviews, coded with either the Reflective Functioning Scale (Fonagy et al., Citation1998) or the Reflective Functioning Scoring Manual (Slade, Grienenberger et al., Citation2005). These interviews elicit representations of attachment relationships and include the Adult Attachment Interview (AAI; George, Kaplan, & Main, Citation1985), the Pregnancy Interview (PI-R, Slade et al., Citation1987), and the Parent Development Interview (PDI; Aber et al., Citation1985). The latter two are primarily used during the third trimester of pregnancy and seek to obtain the reflective quality of expectant parents’ representations of themselves and their unborn baby. Furthermore, Rosenblum et al. (Citation2008) have developed a reflective functioning scale for use with the Working Model of the Child Interview (Zeanah & Benoit, Citation1995). Antenatal mind-mindedness has not been well investigated, but responses to the ‘Describe your child’ question can be coded for spontaneous mental state references (Arnott & Meins, Citation2008).

What is already known about instruments assessing the maternal-fetal relationship?

Recent reviews have highlighted a lack of consensus around definitions of constructs related to the maternal-fetal relationship, as well as heterogeneity across instruments (Cataudella et al., Citation2016; McNamara et al., Citation2019). This may have implications for the consistency of research findings and applicability of assessment tools to clinical practice. Three systematic reviews have evaluated the psychometric properties of instruments used to measure maternal-fetal attachment. Perrelli et al. (Citation2014) reported on four instruments, finding superior construct and criterion validity for the PAI, in comparison to the MAAS, the MFAS and a Portuguese-language questionnaire (Maternal Adjustment and Maternal Attitudes [MAMA]; Figueiredo et al., Citation2014). Van den Bergh and Simons (Citation2009) reviewed the psychometric properties of the MFAS, MAAS, and PAI. It reports on the robustness of each instrument and recommends that their psychometric properties be studied in larger and more varied samples, particularly in populations with cumulative risk factors for antenatal bonding. Most recently, Wittkowski et al. (Citation2020) reviewed perinatal instruments assessing the quality of the maternal-fetal bond, identifying five antenatal measures. They conclude that the PAI is the most psychometrically robust antenatal measure, but that the MFAS was the most accessible based on its readability.

In terms of reviews of measures representing other constructs related to the MFR, Vreeswijk et al. (Citation2012) systematically review evidence on maternal representation as measured by the Working Model of the Child Interview (WMCI), including its adaptation and application antenatally (Benoit et al., Citation1997). However, this does not report specifically on psychometric properties. Similarly, Katznelson (Citation2013) reviewed evidence for the relationship between reflective functioning (including prenatal reflective functioning, assessed with the Pregnancy Interview) and postnatal outcomes relating to the quality of the parent–infant relationship (e.g. Pajulo et al., Citation2012) that again, does not report on psychometric properties.

Thus, work has yet to be done to synthesise what is known about the psychometric properties of all available instruments measuring prenatal maternal representation, reflective functioning and mind-mindedness, alongside available information on measures assessing maternal–fetal-attachment. Further, no review has reported on the feasibility of all of these tools relative to one another, and provided recommendations for their use in clinical practice, which could contribute to their wider use to identify mothers in need of support in the perinatal period.

Summary and aims

Several related antenatal constructs including mind-mindedness, reflective functioning, maternal representation and maternal-fetal attachment are predictive of both parent–infant interaction outcomes and infant developmental outcomes. The conceptual complexity and variety of methodological issues inherent in the research, raises questions regarding which tools are most reliable and valid, as well as having most clinical utility, in terms of identifying expectant mothers in need of support.

This review therefore seeks to identify both self-report and interview-based instruments that would be feasible, reliable and valid for use in clinical settings. Notably, recent updates to measures (e.g. brief report of reflective functioning) may pose advantages in usability and feasibility but potentially at the expense of predictive validity. With view to informing clinician choice on selecting appropriate and feasible instruments to assess the MFR, the research questions are

  1. Which instruments that are used to measure expectant mothers’ thoughts and feelings about their unborn baby have data available regarding their psychometric properties?

  2. How valid and reliable are the above tools, and which have most clinical utility in identifying women in need of additional support during pregnancy?

  3. Based on the above evidence, which instruments(s) for use in in primary and secondary care can be recommended to measure expectant mothers’ thoughts and feelings about their unborn baby?

Methods

The PRISMA statement informed the method and reporting of this review (Liberati et al., Citation2009).

Eligibility criteria

Published research articles reporting the psychometric properties of instruments measuring the MFR in expectant mothers were considered for inclusion in the review. Studies selected for review focused on at least one of the following four constructs: (1) maternal representation, (2) maternal-fetal attachment/bonding, (3) maternal mind-mindedness, and (4) maternal reflective functioning.

The publication date range was from 1995 to the present day because literature reporting on maternal–fetal-attachment has captured relevant data from studies preceding this date, whilst measures reporting on reflective functioning, representation and mind-mindedness were developed and studied in populations of expectant mothers only after 1995. In terms of other inclusion criteria, papers had to be published in English. Papers reporting on the psychometric properties of non-English versions of self-report tools were excluded. Conference abstracts, student dissertations and editorials were also excluded.

Information sources

A systematic literature search was conducted using six electronic databases: Medline, PsychINFO, CINAHL, Scopus, EMCARE and PubMed. Database searches were conducted on 31 March 2020 and updated on 31 March 2021, identifying one further paper meeting the inclusion criteria. Search strategies used both article titles and abstracts and comprised all journal articles published up to this date. The database searches were performed by one author (BIS) and accessed from the library of the University of Oxford. The search terms, used across each database, included

antenatal OR pregnan* OR prenatal OR “expectant mother*” AND “reflective function*” OR representation OR “f*etal attachment” OR “f*etal bond*” OR “mind-mindedness” OR “parent development interview” OR “mother-f*etus relationship” AND ”working model of the child interview” OR “pregnancy interview” OR “interview of maternal representation*” OR “child concept questionnaire” OR “maternal antenatal attachment scale” OR “prenatal marschak interaction method*” OR “prenatal attachment inventory” OR “prenatal parental reflective functioning questionnaire” OR “PDI” OR “PAI” OR “WMCI” OR “MFAS” OR “MAAS” OR “PAAS” OR “CCQ” OR “IRMAG*” AND psychometric* OR reliability OR validity OR consistency

The reference lists of identified review studies (N = 5) were hand-searched, yielding a further seven eligible papers.

Study selection

Abstracts were screened by two independent raters to determine if (1) the study involved a measure of representation, maternal-fetal attachment, mind-mindedness or reflective functioning, (2) the measure assessed these constructs in expectant mothers and (3) the study reported on psychometric data of the measure. The agreement for screening of abstracts was 95% (Cohen’s κ = .83). Conflicts in ratings were then resolved between the raters, to ensure that studies met all three eligibility criteria before proceeding to full-text extraction. One author then screened the full texts against the eligibility criteria.

Data extraction and synthesis

A data extraction form was devised to capture the following parameters: (1) psychometric properties: theoretical framework, reliability, validity, sample characteristics and (2) feasibility of use in a routine clinic setting: length of time of administration, number of items, availability and recommendations for research and clinical use. Where information was missing, study authors were contacted directly. Data extraction was undertaken by a second rater on N = 9 papers, yielding a data replication rate of 89% for sample characteristics (Cohen’s κ = .40) and 96% for psychometric properties (Cohen’s κ = .73). Discrepancies were resolved by discussion to reach consensus. This was an integrative review; results are presented in table format, along with a synthesis of and interpretation of findings in relation to existing literature.

Methodological quality appraisal

The QATSDD (Sirriyeh et al., Citation2012) was used to assess the methodological quality of the studies reviewed. This is a validated 16-item quality assessment instrument shown to have good validity and reliability (κ = 71.5%; Sirriyeh et al., Citation2012). It assesses a range of study designs, evaluating 14 domains relating to methodological quality. All criteria were assessed by one researcher [BIS]. Twelve papers were double-coded for quality by a second rater. Discrepancies (25%; Cohen’s κ = .43) were discussed and resolved to full agreement.

Results

shows the process by which studies were identified. A total of 28 studies featuring 13 tools met the inclusion criteria. Five tools focused on maternal-fetal attachment/bonding, three on maternal representation, three on maternal reflective functioning and one on mind-mindedness.

Figure 1. PRISMA flow chart indicating study selection process.

Figure 1. PRISMA flow chart indicating study selection process.

The sample characteristics of the studies are presented in , and information on results, including psychometric information and feasibility of usage is presented in .

Table 2. Quality of study and characteristics of sample for instruments assessing aspects of the maternal–fetal relationship.

Table 3. Psychometric properties and feasibility of instruments assessing the maternal-fetal relationship.

Study characteristics and quality

The papers identified were published between 1995 and 2019. Samples were recruited from the United States (N = 13), the Netherlands (N = 4), Canada (N = 3), Finland (N = 4), Italy (N = 2), the United Kingdom (N = 1), and Australia (N = 1). Seven studies reported on sample sizes ≤50, 13 on sample sizes ≤200, and 7 on samples >200, with the smallest sample being 30 (Sadler et al., Citation2016) and the largest 666 (Ammaniti et al., Citation2013). Most sample populations comprised expectant mothers in the third trimester of pregnancy (N = 18). Fourteen out of 28 studies included a sample of ‘at-risk’ women. Caucasian participants were heavily represented across studies, with only two studies reporting on a majority of participants from other racial/ethnic groups (Bloom, Citation1997; Sadler et al., Citation2016).

Information on study quality as determined by the QATSDD (Sirriyeh et al., Citation2012) is presented alongside sample characteristics in . Quality ratings demonstrated a trend whereby more recent papers scored higher across all domains. Notably, no studies reported on co-production in relation to either the development of the measure or the study design. Few measures (N = 3) described the statistical consideration of sample size. Psychometric properties were discussed in all studies. Papers reporting on self-report tools presented a greater range and depth of information on psychometric properties due to these either being the main or secondary aim of the study; and papers describing interview measures reported such information in the description of the measure, along with a short comment on inter-rater reliability and/or agreement. No studies reported on predictive validity.

In terms of measures of maternal–fetal attachment, papers reporting on the self-report PAI were of the highest quality (M = 37.5), whilst papers reporting on the MFAS were of the lowest quality (M = 29), reflecting the level of detail and clarity of reporting. Papers reporting on the Pregnancy Interview showed the most variation in quality, with scores ranging from 29 to 36. For example, Sadler et al.’s (Citation2016) small sample size resulted in limitations in terms of representativeness and lacked information on recruitment. Smaling et al. (Citation2015) provided a comprehensive overview of theoretical issues, methodology and results, though (like many) did not report on either sample size calculations or the use of co-production in study design. Pajulo et al. (Citation2015), reporting on the P-PRFQ, and van Bakel et al. (Citation2013), reporting on the PRAM, showed a similar profile.

Psychometric properties

Measures of maternal-fetal attachment

The Maternal-Fetal Attachment Scale ([MFAS], Cranley, Citation1981), Maternal Antenatal Attachment Scale ([MAAS], Condon, Citation1993), Pictorial Representation of Attachment Measure ([PRAM], Van Bakel et al., Citation2013), Prenatal Maternal Attachment Scale ([PMAS], LoBiondo-Wood & Vito-ORourke, Citation1990) and Prenatal Attachment Inventory ([PAI] Muller, Citation1989, Muller & Mercer, Citation1993) were identified in the search.

The MFAS is a 37-item self-report scale, revised to 19-items. It is widely used in clinical research, reporting good reliability across the included studies. Bloom, Citation1997) reports a Cronbach’s coefficient alpha of .88 for the total scale, with subscales – identified through expert consensus – at .73 for role taking, .53 for interaction with fetus, .56 for differentiation from fetus, .55 for giving of self and .77 for attributing characteristics.

The MAAS is a 19-item self-report scale comprising two subscales identified by factor analysis: ‘quality of attachment’ (11-items) and ‘intensity of preoccupation’ (8-items). There is some evidence for construct validity in its correlation with other measures of social wellbeing (Condon & Corkindale, Citation1997), as well as adequate reliability as assessed by internal consistency (Schwerdtfeger & Goff, Citation2007). The lack of norms, categories, or cut-off scores for the MAAS limits its utility in clinical settings.

The PMAS is a 29-item self-report scale, with an additional 10 items available to women who have experienced fetal movement. Reported psychometric properties include good internal consistency (α = .90) and content validity. Currently, the PMAS is used in research settings only.

The PAI is a 29-item with good reliability and validity, including concurrent validity with the Maternal Fetal Attachment Scale, construct validity with Maternal Adjustment and Maternal Attitudes Scale, content validity achieved through review by panel of expert clinicians, theorists and new mothers, and concurrent validity with the Maternal Attachment Interview (Damato, Citation2004). Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) has found that it fits a unidimensional scale (Gau & Lee, Citation2003); the advantage of CFA, as opposed to an exploratory approach, is that it allows for testing of an a priori hypothesis, in keeping with the development of the PAI which is based on theory and literature review.

Finally, the PRAM is a single pictorial representation of the maternal-fetal bond; it showed convergent validity with verbal and non-verbal measures of maternal-fetal attachment assessed with the MAAS. However, as it is a single-item measure, no item analysis or inter-item consistency measures can be performed, nor were other investigations of validity or reliability reported (Van Bakel et al., Citation2013).

Measures of maternal representation

Studies reporting on three interview-based measures of maternal representation were identified: the Working Model of the Child Interview ([WMCI], C. H. Zeanah et al., Citation1994), the Working Model of the Child Interview-D ([WMCI-D] Crawford & Benoit, Citation2009; based on the WMCI; C.H. Zeanah et al., Citation1986), and the Interview of Maternal Representations-Revised ([IRMAG], (Ammaniti & Tambelli, Citation2010). These require between 45 to 60 minutes for delivery, with versions of the WMCI consisting of just 15 items compared with the IRMAG-R which comprises 47 items. One self-report form was also identified (ASQ-IRMAG; Ammaniti et al., Citation1992).

The IRMAG-R is a revised version of the IRMAG interview (Ammaniti et al., Citation1999), with five further questions exploring the influence of the mother’s trauma, fears and preoccupations. The coding system allows the mother’s transcript to be classified as integrated/balanced, restricted/disinvested, or not integrated/ambivalent. Interrater reliability for IRMAG-R scales showed a mean reliability of .92. Interrater reliability for the main category was 94% (Cohen’s κ = .83, p < .001). Factor analyses were reported with principal component analysis showing two factors, indicative of good structural validity. Evidence demonstrates the criterion and incremental validity of IRMAG-R as predictive of postnatal emotional availability and attachment security (Tambelli, Trentini & Dentale, Citation2020). Flykt et al. (Citation2012) report on an adjective scale questionnaire form from the IRMAG: whilst it showed good reliability (κ = .83), no evidence for validity was reported. The authors note the questionnaires are available for research use only.

The WMCI is a 60-minute structured interview examining perceptions and subjective experiences of the current and future relationship between mother and baby. Questions can be changed into the future tense to assess antenatal representations in pregnant women. Percentage agreement for overall classification was between 85% (κ = .57, Benoit et al., Citation1997) and 96% (κ = .62 to .94; p < .001; Dayton et al., Citation2010).

Studies evaluating the properties of the WMCI-D (Huth‐Bocks et al., Citation2004, Citation2011; Lannert et al., Citation2013; Madigan et al., Citation2015; Theran et al., Citation2005; Vreeswijk et al., Citation2015), adapted from the WMCI to capture disorganised representations, have reported on reliability and validity. The WMCI-D shows good inter-rater agreement (90%, κ = .71; Crawford & Benoit, Citation2009) and good construct validity, with an 83% correspondence with the Adult Attachment Interview (Madigan et al., Citation2015). The subscales that were developed for the WMCI-D scale have variable interrater reliability.

Measures of maternal reflective functioning

Three instruments assessing maternal reflective functioning were identified: the P-PRFQ (Pajulo et al., Citation2015), PI (Slade et al; Citation2003; Slade, Grienenberger et al., Citation2005) and PI-R (Slade et al., Citation1987, pp. 2002, 2011).

The PI and PI-R are semi-structured interviews, lasting between 60 and 90 minutes and capturing parents’ reflective functioning capacity. The coding for the PI shows good inter-rater agreement (80%; Sadler et al., Citation2016) and reliability (α = .73 to .95; Salo et al., Citation2019). Construct and structural validity are shown to be robust (Smaling et al., Citation2015, Citation2016). Extensive training on scoring the transcribed interviews is required to access the coding framework. The P-PRFQ (Prenatal Parental Reflective Functioning Questionnaire; Pajulo et al., Citation2010) is a 14-item self-report instrument, designed to address this issue. It shows good reliability (α = .77) and reports both good content and structural validity, with factor analysis demonstrating a three-factor model: ‘Opacity of mental states’, ‘Reflecting on the fetus–child’, and ‘The dynamic nature of mental states’ (Pajulo et al., Citation2015).

Measures of maternal mind-mindedness

Mind-mindedness has not been well studied in populations of expectant mothers. Only one study was identified that used a measure of mind-mindedness in the antenatal period (Arnott & Meins, Citation2008). This asked mothers an adaptation of the ‘Describe your Child’ question typically used to assess mind-mindedness, assessing the degree to which expectant mothers use mentalising, behavioural, physical or general descriptors to predict what their child will be like. Coding with the mind-mindedness coding manual (Meins & Fernyhough, Citation2006) showed good inter-rater reliability (κ = .94). Although the construct validity of antenatal mind-mindedness was not demonstrated, it showed good discriminant validity with respect to the MAAS (Condon, Citation1993). The measure is currently only recommended for research use.

Feasibility for use within clinical settings

The usability of the tools within clinical or research settings varied. An overview of factors relating to usability is provided in . The majority of instruments (N = 10) were designated suitable for use in both clinical and research settings. The number of items for self-report tools ranged from 14 (P-PRFQ; Pajulo et al., Citation2010) to 39 (PMAS; LoBiondo-Wood & Vito-O’Rourke, 1990). For interview-based tools, it ranged from 15 (WMCI; C. H. Zeanah et al., Citation1994) to 47 (IRMAG-R; Ammaniti & Tambelli, Citation2010); the time for administration was longest for the Pregnancy Interview (Slade et al., Citation1987, pp. 2002, 2005) at up to 90 minutes, and shortest for the IRMAG-R (Ammaniti & Tambelli, Citation2010) at 45–60 minutes. The majority of tools, whether self-report or clinician-rated, used a response or coding format of Likert scales (N = 11). Seven tools were self-report instruments, while the remaining six were clinician-rated, using coding manuals that require specialist training to administer. Whilst most tools could be located online and were free to use, the Pregnancy Interview coding scheme is only provided to practitioners trained in its application.

Discussion

The purpose of this systematic review was to identify and evaluate the quality and availability of contemporary measures of representation, fetal attachment/bonding, mind-mindedness and reflective functioning as applied in populations of expectant mothers, including their psychometric properties. The measurement of these constructs is important for policymakers as they serve as strong predictors of later difficulties in the parent–infant relationship and later infant outcomes (e.g. Branjerdporn et al., Citation2017), thereby providing an important window of opportunity in terms of prevention.

What tools are used to measure the maternal-fetal relationship?

Thirteen tools were identified, each measuring one of the four central theoretical constructs relating to the MFR. The majority of instruments measured attachment/bonding (N = 5), followed by representation (N = 4), reflective functioning (N = 3) and finally mind-mindedness (N = 1).

How valid and reliable are tools used to measure the maternal-fetal relationship?

The papers identified for review varied in the extent to which they reported on instrument reliability and validity, highlighting limitations in the availability of psychometric information. For example, the MFAS (Cranley, Citation1981), whilst widely used in clinical research studies, shows significant gaps in the evaluation and reporting of validity. Previous reviews (e.g. Beck, Citation1999; Doan, Cox & Zimmerman, Citation2003; Van den Bergh & Simons, Citation2009) note longstanding issues with both content validity and the method through which the dimensions were constructed, which was primarily through expert perspective as opposed to statistical methods.

Of the tools assessing MFA, studies using the PAI (Muller, Citation1989, Muller & Mercer, Citation1993) demonstrated the strongest evidence for reliability and validity across a range of domains. One issue identified across MFA tools was the relative lack of known-groups validity – that is, differentiation between community and at-risk samples – compared to tools measuring reflective functioning and representation. Further research is needed to identify cut-off scores and criteria for raising concerns about the developing maternal-fetal bond. Of instruments assessing maternal representation, studies reporting on the WMCI (C. H. Zeanah et al., Citation1994) showed the most substantial evidence for reliability as well as demonstrating content, structural, criterion and construct validity. The Adjective Scale Questionnaire from the IRMAG also showed promise in this regard, although would benefit from further research. Finally, regarding antenatal mentalisation, studies using the Pregnancy Interview (Slade et al., 2002) reported most robust evidence for reliability and validity. Meanwhile, initial investigations of the P-PRFQ (Pajulo et al., Citation2010) indicated potential with respect to its psychometric properties. Measurement of antenatal mind-mindedness has only been reported in one study, which was limited in its reporting of psychometric data. Further investigation is required before it can be recommended for use in clinical settings.

What is the feasibility of tools in identifying mothers in need of further support?

The feasibility of using each tool was defined in relation to a number of different factors (e.g. number of items, time to administer, self-report or clinician-rated, suitability for use in clinical settings and availability). Mothers considered at highest risk include those who have experienced childhood maltreatment (Malone et al., Citation2010) and domestic violence (e.g. Huth‐Bocks et al., Citation2004; Lannert et al., Citation2013). Other risk factors include detrimental health behaviour during pregnancy (Fares & Adler, Citation1998), the presence of two or more children under 7 years in the household (Pajulo et al., Citation2006), poor marital adaptation (Sitrin, Citation2001; Fares & Adler, Citation1998) and maternal education, social support, and substance use (Smaling et al., Citation2016).

Brief self-report tools are most suitable for use in primary care settings. For example, the 29-item PAI (Muller, Citation1989, Muller & Mercer, Citation1993) performs well in psychometric terms, presents minimal participant burden, and is easily scored by clinicians. In contrast, interview-based measures may be less well suited to screening, requiring application of a coding scheme that is both costly and resource-intensive. Unfortunately, this has implications for the assessment of maternal representation, reflective functioning and mind-mindedness and raises questions about whether and why these constructs are under-represented in the literature compared to maternal-fetal attachment. The 14-item self-report tool, the P-PRFQ (Pajulo et al., Citation2010), measuring reflective functioning, may present a viable alternative. However, having been developed more recently, it is still in the early stages of validation and reliability testing. Further investigation is needed to confirm its utility and robustness as a screening tool.

Implications and recommendations for clinical practice

Whilst there is no literature directly comparing the predictive validity of different MFR tools, previous research demonstrates a relationship between psychosocial risk factors, constructs related to the MFR, and postnatal outcomes. This includes evidence for the detrimental impact of maternal risk factors on quality of antenatal representation (Vreeswijk et al., Citation2012, Citation2015), reflective functioning (Huth‐Bocks et al., Citation2004; Smaling et al., Citation2015; Theran et al., Citation2005), and maternal-fetal attachment, with further implications for their association with postnatal bonding (McNamara et al., Citation2019). The accurate identification of at-risk mothers provides an important window of opportunity to deliver targeted support to reduce the likelihood of adverse child outcomes. Further research should identify which construct, and indeed which measure, is most strongly associated with parent–infant interaction quality and infant outcomes.

Findings from this review suggest that self-report instruments, such as the PAI and MAAS, can be recommended for use in assessing maternal-fetal attachment in primary care settings (see ). Both show good psychometric properties and usability, with higher scores indicating a stronger attachment bond. Worth noting is that self-report measures may be less well-suited for at-risk populations where reflexivity, self-awareness and insight may be compromised. The high face validity of these measures may result in expectant mothers over-reporting their degree of bonding with their unborn baby, particularly in contexts where they may feel stigmatised. Following further investigation into its psychometric profile, the self-report P-PRFQ may be a more suitable, less transparent measure for screening levels of reflective functioning. Moreover, as a relatively brief measure that does not require specialist training to administer, it can be used by practitioners such as midwives, health visitors and public health nurses in the third trimester of pregnancy. Importantly, it should be noted that in the absence of cut-off scores and norms for self-report instruments, where lower scores indicate concerns within the MFR, clinicians should also utilise structured professional judgement to identify mothers in need of further support. Accordingly, scores on self-report measures should be interpreted alongside any pre-existing risk factors. Where lower scores on self-report measures are present, alongside other risk factors, onward referral may be necessary to tertiary services that have the resources to utilise interview measures and can offer further intervention.

Table 4. Recommendations for instruments to be used in primary and secondary care settings.

In such specialist settings, the Pregnancy Interview Revised should be used to ascertain reflective functioning capacities. This interview would not only help to clarify the level of support needed but also has the potential to facilitate reflexivity by helping the mother to consider her thoughts and feelings towards her unborn baby, thus acting as an intervention in itself. These women could then be directed towards appropriate resources (e.g. the Getting to Know Your Baby programme, www.your-baby.org.uk), advice around techniques to improve the developing relationship, in addition to early intervention services. These might include mentalisation-based interventions, such as Minding the Baby (Sadler et al., Citation2013), which may be beneficial for women with more challenging psychosocial histories and contexts (e.g. domestic violence, drug misuse, experience of trauma).

Strengths and limitations

To our knowledge, this is the first review to synthesise what is known about the psychometric and clinimetric properties of instruments measuring the quality of the maternal-fetal relationship across all four theoretical constructs, providing recommendations for use in clinical practice. One limitation of this systematic review is the possibility that not all instruments for evaluating the MFR were identified or included. There is also potential for gaps in information on psychometric properties owing to the exclusion of papers reporting on translations of self-report tools. For example, studies reporting on Polish and Swedish translations of the PAI yield a five-factor structure based on principal component analysis (Bielawska‐Batorowicz & Siddiqui, Citation2008; Siddiqui et al., Citation2000) as opposed to the single-factor model, reported in the present paper, derived from confirmatory factor analysis (Gau & Lee, Citation2003).

Additionally, a comprehensive evaluation of other areas of feasibility has not been reported, including information regarding required reading level and length/cost of training for interview-based measures. Unfortunately, this information was not available in any paper selected for review, although it is an important aspect of feasibility. Similarly, despite efforts to contact authors of instruments included in this review, the authors of the IRMAG-R could not be reached to ascertain its availability and cost. Finally, whilst the QATSSD was chosen as a quality-rating tool because it has the scope to assess studies of a range of methodologies included in the review, there is a need for the development and evaluation of quality rating tools that specifically assess studies reporting on both psychometric and usability data.

Recommendations for further research and clinical practice

Further research should continue to evaluate the psychometric properties, availability and feasibility of using these tools such that clearer comparisons can be made of their psychometric properties where information is currently lacking. The development of the P-PRFQ is promising with a view to broadening the study of antenatal reflective functioning where the resource-intensive use of interview-based instruments might otherwise limit the scope of its application. Work should also be undertaken to compare the predictive validity and strength of association between instruments measuring each of the four constructs and postnatal parent–infant outcomes, with a view to establishing which are the most helpful in identifying mothers in need of support. Additional efforts should be made not only in early identification but also to ensure that such support is both available and accessible. Services may want to consider a stepped care model to demarcate appropriate care pathways, with psychoeducational materials, guided self-help and support groups followed by more intensive provision such as mentalisation-based treatment programmes (Sadler et al., Citation2013). Accordingly, future directions for research include establishing clear clinical cut-offs and criteria within these measures to identify mothers at-risk of experiencing difficulties in their relationship with their unborn baby.

Conclusion

The quality of the maternal-fetal relationship has important implications for parent–infant interaction and infant outcomes, which in turn have significant longer-term effects on child development. This suggests a need for further research into instruments measuring the MFR that can be used to enrich current understanding of risk and transmission pathways. Advances in this understanding represent an important opportunity to anticipate risk of difficulties in the parent–infant relationship and thereby offer preventative intervention to disrupt the intergenerational transmission of attachment difficulties and scaffold positive outcomes for child socio-emotional development. Yet, this possibility depends on successful translation of theoretical understanding into clinical practice. The present review contributes recommendations for robust, clinically feasible instruments that can be used by clinicians to identify expectant mothers in need of further support.

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).

References

  • Aber, J. L., Slade, A., Berger, B., Bresgi, I., & Kaplan, M. (1985). The Parent Development Interview. Unpublished manuscript.
  • Ammaniti, M., Baumgartner, E., Candelori, C., Perucchini, P., Pola, M., Tambelli, R., & Zampino, F. (1992). Representations and narratives during pregnancy. Infant Mental Health Journal, 13(2), 167–182. https://doi.org/10.1002/1097-0355(199223)13:2<167::AID-IMHJ2280130207>3.0.CO;2-M
  • Ammaniti, M., Candelori, M., Pola, R., & Tambelli, R. (1999). Maternité et grossesse [Maternity and pregnancy]. Presses Universitaries de France.
  • Ammaniti, M., Tambelli, R., & Perucchini, P. (2000). De la Grossesse à la période post-accouchement: Stabilité et évolution des représentations maternelles. Devenir (Genève, 12(2), 57–74.
  • Ammaniti, M., & Tambelli, R. (2010). Prenatal self-report questionnaires, scales and interviews. In S. Tyano, M. Keren, H. Herman, & J. Cox (Eds.), Parenthood and mental health. A bridge between infant and adult psychiatry (pp. 109–120). Wiley-Blackwell.
  • Ammaniti, M., Tambelli, R., & Odorisio, F. (2013). Exploring maternal representations during pregnancy in normal and at-risk samples: The use of the interview of maternal representations during pregnancy. Infant Mental Health Journal, 34(1), 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.21357
  • Arnott, B., & Meins, E. (2007). Links among antenatal attachment representations, postnatal mind–mindedness, and infant attachment security: A preliminary study of mothers and fathers. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 71(2), 132–149. https://doi.org/10.1521/bumc.2007.71.2.132
  • Arnott, B., & Meins, E. (2007). Links between antenatal attachment representations, postnatal mind-mindedness, and infant attachment security: A preliminary study of mothers and fathers. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 71(2), 132–149.
  • Arnott, B., & Meins, E. (2008). Continuity in mind-mindedness from pregnancy to the first year of life. Infant Behavior and Development, 31(4), 647–654. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2008.07.001
  • Atkinson, L., Leung, E., Goldberg, S., Benoit, D., Poulton, L., Myhal, N., Kerr, S., & Kerr, S. (2009). Attachment and selective attention: Disorganization and emotional stroop reaction time. Development and Psychopathology, 21(1), 99–126. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579409000078
  • Beck, C. T. (1999). Available instruments for research on prenatal attachment and adaptation to pregnancy. MCN: The American Journal of Maternal/Child Nursing, 24(1), 25–32. https://doi.org/10.1097/00005721-199901000-00006
  • Benoit, D., Parker, K. C., & Zeanah, C. H. (1997). Mothers’ representations of their infants assessed prenatally: Stability and association with infants’ attachment classifications. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38(3), 307–313. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01515.x
  • Bielawska‐Batorowicz, E., & Siddiqui, A. (2008). A study of prenatal attachment with Swedish and Polish expectant mothers. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology, 26(4), 373–384. https://doi.org/10.1080/02646830802426144
  • Bloom, K. C. (1997). The development of attachment behaviors in pregnant adolescents. MCN: The American Journal of Maternal/Child Nursing, 22(1), 8.
  • Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss v. 3 (1). Random House.
  • Bowlby, J. (1982). Attachment and loss: Retrospect and prospect. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 52(4), 664. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1939-0025.1982.tb01456.x
  • Branjerdporn, G., Meredith, P., Strong, J., & Garcia, J. (2017). Associations between maternal-foetal attachment and infant developmental outcomes: A systematic review. Maternal and Child Health Journal, 21(3), 540–553. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10995-016-2138-2
  • Cataudella, S., Lampis, J., Busonera, A., Marino, L., & Zavattini, G. C. (2016). From parental-fetal attachment to a parent-infant relationship: A systematic review about prenatal protective and risk factors. Life Span and Disability, 19(2),185–219.
  • Cohen, L. J., & Slade, A. (1999). The psychology and psychopathology of pregnancy: Reorganization and transformation. In J. C. H. Zeanah (Ed.), Handbook of infant mental health 2000. (pp. 20–36). The Guilford Press.
  • Condon, J. T. (1993). The assessment of antenatal emotional attachment: Development of a questionnaire instrument. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 66(2), 167–183. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8341.1993.tb01739.x
  • Condon, J. T., & Corkindale, C. (1997). The correlates of antenatal attachment in pregnant women. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 70(4), 359–372. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8341.1997.tb01912.x
  • Cranley, M. S. (1981). Development of a tool for the measurement of maternal attachment during pregnancy. Nursing Research, 30(5), 281???284. https://doi.org/10.1097/00006199-198109000-00008
  • Crawford, A., & Benoit, D. (2009). Caregivers’ disrupted representations of the unborn child predict later infant–caregiver disorganized attachment and disrupted interactions. Infant Mental Health Journal: Official Publication of the World Association for Infant Mental Health, 30(2), 124–144. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.20207
  • Damato, E. G. (2004). Prenatal attachment and other correlates of postnatal maternal attachment to twins. Advances in Neonatal Care, 4(5), 274–291. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adnc.2004.07.005
  • Dayton, C. J., Levendosky, A. A., Davidson, W. S., & Bogat, G. A. (2010). The child as held in the mind of the mother: The influence of prenatal maternal representations on parenting behaviors. Infant Mental Health Journal: Official Publication of the World Association for Infant Mental Health, 31(2), 220–241. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.20253
  • Doan, H. M., Cox, N. L., & Zimmerman, A. (2003). The maternal fetal attachment scale: Some methodological ponderings. Journal of Prenatal & Perinatal Psychology & Health, 18, 167–188.
  • Fares, S. L., & Adler, N. E. (1998). Top-of-the-head’descriptions of baby and self: Links to reported health behaviors and social context in early pregnancy. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 15(4), 555–564. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407598154007
  • Figueiredo, B., Tendais, I., & Dias, C. C. (2014). Maternal adjustment and maternal attitudes in adolescent and adult pregnant women. Journal of Pediatric and Adolescent Gynecology, 27(4), 194–201. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpag.2013.09.014
  • Flykt, M., Punamäki, R. L., Belt, R., Biringen, Z., Salo, S., Posa, T., & Pajulo, M. (2012). Maternal representations and emotional availability among drug‐abusing and nonusing mothers and their infants. Infant Mental Health Journal, 33(2), 123–138. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.21313
  • Foley, S., & Hughes, C. (2018). Great expectations? Do mothers’ and fathers’ prenatal thoughts and feelings about the infant predict parent-infant interaction quality? A meta-analytic review. Developmental Review, 48, 40–54. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2018.03.007
  • Fonagy, P., Steele, M., Steele, H., Moran, G. S., & Higgitt, A. C. (1991). The capacity for understanding mental states: The reflective self in parent and child and its significance for security of attachment. Infant Mental Health Journal, 12(3), 201–218. https://doi.org/10.1002/1097-0355(199123)12:3<201::AID-IMHJ2280120307>3.0.CO;2-7
  • Fonagy, P., Steele, M., Steele, H., Leigh, T., Kennedy, R., Mattoon, G., & Target, M. (1995). Attachment, the reflective self, and borderline states: The predictive specificity of the adult attachment interview and pathological emotional development. In S. Goldberg, R. Muir, & J. Kerr (Eds.), Attachment theory: Social, developmental, and clinical perspectives (pp. 233–278). Analytic Press.
  • Fonagy, P., Target, M., Steele, H., & Steele, M. (1998). Reflective-functioning manual, version 5.0, for application to adult attachment interviews ( pp. 161–162). University College London.
  • Fonagy, P., Luyten, P., Moulton-Perkins, A., Lee, Y. W., Warren, F., Howard, S., Lowyck, B., Lowyck, B., & Ghinai, R. (2016). Development and validation of a self-report measure of mentalizing: The reflective functioning questionnaire. PLoS One, 11(7), e0158678. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0158678
  • Fowles, E. R. (1996). Relationships among prenatal maternal attachment, presence of postnatal depressive symptoms, and maternal role attainment. Journal for Specialists in Pediatric Nursing, 1(2), 75–82.
  • Gau, M. L., & Lee, T. Y. (2003). Construct validity of the prenatal attachment inventory: A confirmatory factor analysis approach. The Journal of Nursing Research: JNR, 11(3), 177–187. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.JNR.0000347634.18218.2f
  • George, C., Kaplan, N., & Main, M. (1985). The Berkeley adult attachment interview. Unpublished protocol, Department of Psychology, University of California.
  • Gloger-Tippelt, G. (1983). A process model of the pregnancy course. Human Development, 26(3), 134–148. https://doi.org/10.1159/000272877
  • Huth‐Bocks, A. C., Levendosky, A. A., Theran, S. A., & Bogat, G. A. (2004). The impact of domestic violence on mothers’ prenatal representations of their infants. Infant Mental Health Journal: Official Publication of the World Association for Infant Mental Health, 25(2), 79–98. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.10094
  • Huth‐Bocks, A. C., Theran, S. A., Levendosky, A. A., & Bogat, G. A. (2011). A social‐contextual understanding of concordance and discordance between maternal prenatal representations of the infant and infant–mother attachment. Infant Mental Health Journal, 32(4), 405–426. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.20304
  • Katznelson, H. (2014). Reflective functioning: A review. Clinical Psychology Review, 34(2), 107–117.
  • Korja, R., Ahlqvist-Björkroth, S., Savonlahti, E., Stolt, S., Haataja, L., Lapinleimu, H., Piha, J., & Lehtonen, L., & PIPARI Study Group. (2010). Relations between maternal attachment representations and the quality of mother–infant interaction in preterm and full-term infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 33(3), 330–336. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2010.03.010
  • Lannert, B. K., Levendosky, A. A., & Bogat, G. A. (2013). The interaction of maternal personality traits and intimate partner violence as influences on maternal representations. Infant Mental Health Journal, 34(3), 222–233. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.21385
  • Le Bas, G. A., Youssef, G. J., Macdonald, J. A., Rossen, L., Teague, S. J., Kothe, E. J., McIntosh, J. E., Olsson, C. A., & Hutchinson, D. M. (2020). The role of antenatal and postnatal maternal bonding in infant development: A systematic review and meta‐analysis. Social Development, 29(1), 3–20. https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12392
  • Liberati, A., Altman, D. G., Tetzlaff, J., Mulrow, C., Gøtzsche, P. C., Ioannidis, J. P., Clarke, M., Devereaux, P. J., Kleijnen, J., & Moher, D. (2009). The PRISMA statement for reporting systematic reviews and meta-analyses of studies that evaluate health care interventions: Explanation and elaboration. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 62(10), e1–e34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclinepi.2009.06.006
  • LoBiondo-Wood, G., & Vito O'Rourke, K. (1990). The Prenatal Maternal Attachment Scale: A methodological study. In NAACOG’s Research Conference, Denver, CO, USA (pp. 20–21).
  • Madigan, S., Hawkins, E., Plamondon, A., Moran, G., & Benoit, D. (2015). Maternal representations and infant attachment: An examination of the prototype hypothesis. Infant Mental Health Journal, 36(5), 459–468. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.21527
  • Malone, J. C., Levendosky, A. A., Dayton, C. J., & Bogat, G. A. (2010). Understanding the “ghosts in the nursery” of pregnant women experiencing domestic violence: Prenatal maternal representations and histories of childhood maltreatment. Infant Mental Health Journal, 31(4), 432–454. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.20264
  • McMahon, C. A., & Bernier, A. (2017). Twenty years of research on parental mind-mindedness: Empirical findings, theoretical and methodological challenges, and new directions. Developmental Review, 46, 54–80. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2017.07.001
  • McNamara, J., Townsend, M. L., Herbert, J. S., & Hill, B. (2019). A systemic review of maternal wellbeing and its relationship with maternal fetal attachment and early postpartum bonding. PloS one, 14(7), e0220032. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0220032
  • Meins, E. (1997). Security of attachment and maternal tutoring strategies: Interaction within the zone of proximal development. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 15(2), 129–144. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.1997.tb00730.x
  • Meins, E. (1998). The effects of security of attachment and material attribution of meaning on children’s linguistic acquisitional style. Infant Behavior and Development, 21(2), 237–252. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(98)90004-2
  • Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Fradley, E., & Tuckey, M. (2001). Rethinking maternal sensitivity: Mothers’ comments on infants’ mental processes predict security of attachment at 12 months. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 42(5), 637–648. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00759
  • Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Russell, J., & Clark-Carter, D. (2001). Security of attachment as a predictor of symbolic and mentalising abilities: A longitudinal study. Social Development, 7(1), 1–24. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9507.00047
  • Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Wainwright, R., Das Gupta, M., Fradley, E., & Tuckey, M. (2002). Maternal mind–mindedness and attachment security as predictors of theory of mind understanding. Child Development, 73(6), 1715–1726. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00501
  • Meins, E., & Fernyhough, C. (2006). Mind-mindedness coding manual. Unpublished manuscript. Durham University, Durham, UK, 82.
  • Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., de Rosnay, M., Arnott, B., Leekam, S. R., & Turner, M. (2012). Mind‐mindedness as a multidimensional construct: Appropriate and nonattuned mind‐related comments independently predict infant–mother attachment in a socially diverse sample. Infancy, 17(4), 393–415. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7078.2011.00087.x
  • Mercer, R. T., & Ferketich, S. L. (1995). Experienced and inexperienced mothers’ maternal competence during infancy. Research in Nursing & Health, 18(4), 333–343. https://doi.org/10.1002/nur.4770180407
  • Muller, M. E. (1989). The development and testing of the Müller Prenatal Attachment Inventory. University of California.
  • Muller, M. E., & Mercer, R. T. (1993). Development of the prenatal attachment inventory. Western Journal of Nursing Research, 15(2), 199–215. https://doi.org/10.1177/019394599301500205
  • Oppenheim, D., & Koren‐Karie, N. (2002). Mothers’ insightfulness regarding their children’s internal worlds: The capacity underlying secure child–mother relationships. Infant Mental Health Journal: Official Publication of the World Association for Infant Mental Health, 23(6), 593–605. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.10035
  • Pajulo, M., Suchman, N., Kalland, M., & Mayes, L. (2006). Enhancing the effectiveness of residential treatment for substance abusing pregnant and parenting women: Focus on maternal reflective functioning and mother‐child relationship. Infant Mental Health Journal: Official Publication of the World Association for Infant Mental Health, 27(5), 448–465. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.20100
  • Pajulo, M., Pyykkönen, N., Kalland, M., Sinkkonen, J., Helenius, H., Punamäki, R. L., & Suchman, N. (2012). Substance‐abusing mothers in residential treatment with their babies: Importance of pre‐and postnatal maternal reflective functioning. Infant Mental Health Journal, 33(1), 70–81. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.20342
  • Pajulo, M., Tolvanen, M., Karlsson, L., Halme‐Chowdhury, E., Öst, C., Luyten, P., Karlsson, L., Mayes, L., & Karlsson, H. (2015). The prenatal parental reflective functioning questionnaire: Exploring factor structure and construct validity of a new measure in The Finn Brain birth cohort pilot study. Infant Mental Health Journal, 36(4), 399–414. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.21523
  • Pajulo, M., Tolvanen, M., Karlsson, L., Halme‐chowdhury, E., Öst, C., Luyten, P., & Karlsson, H. (2015). The Prenatal Parental Reflective Functioning Questionnaire: Exploring factor structure and construct validity of a new measure in the Finn brain birth cohort pilot study. Infant Mental Health Journal, 36(4), 399–414. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.21523
  • Perrelli, J. G. A., Zambaldi, C. F., Cantilino, A., & Sougey, E. B. (2014). Mother-child bonding assessment tools. Revista Paulista de Pediatria (English Edition), 32(3), 257–265. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2359-3482(15)30020-8
  • Rosenblum, K. L., McDonough, S. C., Sameroff, A. J., & Muzik, M. (2008). Reflection in thought and action: Maternal parenting reflectivity predicts mind‐minded comments and interactive behavior. Infant Mental Health Journal: Official Publication of the World Association for Infant Mental Health, 29(4), 362–376. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.20184
  • Rosenblum, K. L., Dayton, C. J., & Muzik, M. (2009). Infant social and emotional development: Emerging competence in a relational context. In C. H. Zeanah Jr. (Ed.), Handbook of infant mental health (3rd ed., pp. 80–103). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
  • Sadler, L. S., Slade, A., Close, N., Webb, D. L., Simpson, T., Fennie, K., & Mayes, L. C. (2013). Minding the baby: Enhancing reflectiveness to improve early health and relationship outcomes in an interdisciplinary home‐visiting program. Infant Mental Health Journal, 34(5), 391–405. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.21406
  • Sadler, L. S., Novick, G., & Meadows-Oliver, M. (2016). “Having a baby changes everything” reflective functioning in pregnant adolescents. Journal of Pediatric Nursing, 31(3), e219–e231. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pedn.2015.11.011
  • Salo, S. J., Flykt, M., Isosävi, S., Punamäki, R. L., Kalland, M., Biringen, Z., & Pajulo, M. (2019). Validating an observational measure of prenatal emotional availability among mothers with depressive symptoms. Journal of Prenatal & Perinatal Psychology & Health, 34(1), 55–77. http://hdl.handle.net/10138/308936
  • Schwerdtfeger, K. L., & Goff, B. S. N. (2007). Intergenerational transmission of trauma: Exploring mother–infant prenatal attachment. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 20(1), 39–51. https://doi.org/10.1002/jts.20179
  • Siddiqui, A., & Hägglöf, B. (2000). Does maternal prenatal attachment predict postnatal mother–infant interaction? Early Human Development, 59(1), 13–25. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0378-3782(00)00076-1
  • Siddiqui, A., Hagglof, B., & Eisemann, M. (2000). Own memories of upbringing as a determinant of prenatal attachment in expectant women. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology, 18(1), 67–74. https://doi.org/10.1080/02646830050001690
  • Sirriyeh, R., Lawton, R., Gardner, P., & Armitage, G. (2012). Reviewing studies with diverse designs: The development and evaluation of a new tool. Journal of Evaluation in Clinical Practice, 18(4), 746–752. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2753.2011.01662.x
  • Sitrin, A. G. (2001). The impact of the quality of marital adaptation on prenatal maternal representations and postnatal satisfaction with social support. City University of New York.
  • Slade, A., Huganir, L., Grunebaum, L., & Reeves, M. (1987). The pregnancy interview. In Unpublished manuscript. The City University of New York.
  • Slade, A., Grunebaum, L., Huganir, L., & Reeves, M. (2003). The pregnancy interview. Unpublished manuscript. Yale Child study center.
  • Slade, A., Grienenberger, J., Bernbach, E., Levy, D., & Locker, A. (2005). Maternal reflective functioning, attachment, and the transmission gap: A preliminary study. Attachment & Human Development, 7(3), 283–298. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616730500245880
  • Smaling, H. J., Huijbregts, S. C., Suurland, J., Van Der Heijden, K. B., Van Goozen, S. H., & Swaab, H. (2015). Prenatal reflective functioning in primiparous women with a high‐risk profile. Infant Mental Health Journal, 36(3), 251–261. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.21506
  • Smaling, H. J., Huijbregts, S. C. J., Van der Heijden, K. B., Van Goozen, S. H. M., & Swaab, H. (2016). Maternal reflective functioning as a multidimensional construct: Differential associations with children’s temperament and externalizing behavior. Infant Behavior and Development, 44, 263–274. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2016.06.007
  • Stern, D. (1995). The motherhood constellation: A unified view of parent-infant psychotherapy. Basic Books.
  • Tambelli, R., Trentini, C., & Dentale, F. (2020). Predictive and incremental validity of parental representations during pregnancy on child attachment. Frontiers in Public Health, 8, 439449.
  • Theran, S. A., Levendosky, A. A., Anne Bogat, G., & Huth-Bocks, A. C. (2005). Stability and change in mothers’ internal representations of their infants over time. Attachment & Human Development, 7(3), 253–268. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616730500245609
  • van Bakel, H. J., Maas, A. J. B., Vreeswijk, C. M., & Vingerhoets, A. J. (2013). Pictorial representation of attachment: Measuring the parent-fetus relationship in expectant mothers and fathers. BMC Pregnancy and Childbirth, 13(1), 138. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2393-13-138
  • Van den Bergh, B., & Simons, A. (2009). A review of scales to measure the mother–foetus relationship. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology, 27(2), 114–126. https://doi.org/10.1080/02646830802007480
  • Vreeswijk, C. M., Maas, A. J. B., & van Bakel, H. J. (2012). Parental representations: A systematic review of the working model of the child interview. Infant Mental Health Journal, 33(3), 314–328. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.20337
  • Vreeswijk, C. M., Rijk, C. H., Maas, A. J. B., & Van Bakel, H. J. (2015). Fathers’ and mothers’ representations of the infant: Association with prenatal risk factors. Infant Mental Health Journal, 36(6), 599–612. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.21541
  • Wittkowski, A., Vatter, S., Muhinyi, A., Garrett, C., & Henderson, M. (2020). Measuring bonding or attachment in the parent-infant-relationship: A systematic review of parent-report assessment measures, their psychometric properties and clinical utility. Clinical Psychology Review, 82, 1–27. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2020.101906
  • Zeanah, C. H., Benoit, D., & Barton, M. L. (1986). Working Model of the Child Interview. Unpublished manuscript, Brown University Program in Medicine.
  • Zeanah, C. H., Benoit, D., Hirshberg, L., Barton, M. L., & Regan, C. (1994). Mothers’ representations of their infants are concordant with infant attachment classifications. Developmental Issues in Psychiatry and Psychology, 1(1), 1–14.
  • Zeanah, C. H., & Benoit, D. (1995). Working model of the child interview (WMCI). Tulane University School of Medicine.
  • Zeanah, C. H. (2000). Disturbances of attachment in young children adopted from institutions. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 21(3), 230–236.
  • Zeegers, M. A., Colonnesi, C., Stams, G. J. J., & Meins, E. (2017). Mind matters: A meta-analysis on parental mentalization and sensitivity as predictors of infant–parent attachment. Psychological Bulletin, 143(12), 1245. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000114