Publication Cover
Stress
The International Journal on the Biology of Stress
Volume 26, 2023 - Issue 1
2,677
Views
1
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Short Communication

Inverse association between stress induced cortisol elevations and negative emotional reactivity to stress in humans

, ORCID Icon, , , , , , & show all
Article: 2174780 | Received 02 Sep 2022, Accepted 25 Jan 2023, Published online: 11 Feb 2023

Abstract

Greater cortisol reactivity to stress is often assumed to lead to heightened negative affective reactivity to stress. Conversely, a growing body of evidence demonstrates mood-protective effects of cortisol elevations in the context of acute stress. We administered a laboratory-based stressor, the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST), and measured cortisol and emotional reactivity in 68 adults (48 women) between the ages of 25 and 65. In accordance with our pre-registered hypothesis (https://osf.io/t8r3w) and prior research, negative affective reactivity was inversely related to cortisol reactivity assessed immediately after the stressor. We found that greater cortisol response to acute stress is associated with smaller increases in negative affect, consistent with mood-protective effects of cortisol elevations in response to acute stress.

The relationship between the cortisol and the emotional response to stress has long been a topic of great interest (Het et al., Citation2012). A difficult topic to study naturalistically, “stressful” situations induce elevations in both glucocorticoids (i.e. cortisol in humans) and negative affect, such as anxiety or feeling threatened (Kirschbaum & Hellhammer, Citation1994). Because of the situational correspondence between cortisol elevation and negative affect, the magnitude of cortisol and negative affective stress responses have been assumed to be positively correlated.

Studies that have systematically examined the relationship between emotion and cortisol elevation during stress have produced mixed results (Hellhammer & Schubert, Citation2012; Het et al., Citation2012). In fact, multiple studies investigating acute stressors demonstrate mood-protective effects of cortisol (Het et al., Citation2012; Nakataki et al., Citation2017; Soravia et al., Citation2006). For instance, pharmacological administration of cortisol prior to a stressor has anxiolytic or mood buffering effects in multiple placebo-controlled studies (Het & Wolf, Citation2007; Nakataki et al., Citation2017; Soravia et al., Citation2006). In addition, an investigation that pooled participants across multiple studies employing the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST) (Kirschbaum et al., Citation1993) demonstrated an inverse relationship between stress-induced cortisol and negative affect (Het et al., Citation2012). Thus, evidence is accruing to support mood-protective effects of both exogenous and endogenous cortisol elevation in relation to acute stress.

Mood-protective effects of transient cortisol elevations contrast with pervasive notions of cortisol’s detrimental effects. Modern conceptualizations are dominated by knowledge of the deleterious effects of chronic cortisol elevations (McEwen, Citation2019). The psychologically adaptive effects of transient cortisol elevations in response to acute stress are less well-known (McEwen, Citation2019).

We conducted a TSST as part of a larger NIH-funded project examining individual differences in emotional and stress responses. Our goal is the targeted analysis of cortisol and affective reactivity to the TSST. We hypothesized that we would replicate the inverse relationship between cortisol and negative affective responses to the TSST observed in prior research (Het et al., Citation2012).

Method

Participants

Participants were recruited through online postings, flyers, and emailed invitations to University of Wisconsin-Madison employees and Survey of the Health of Wisconsin participants. Informed consent was obtained from 77 participants between the ages of 25–65 (mean age ± SD = 40.4 ± 12.9; 48 women, 28 men, 1 nonbinary; 3 American Indian, 6 Asian, 5 Black, 3 multiracial, and 60 white; 4 Hispanic, 73 non-Hispanic). Exclusionary criteria included medication changes in the previous 4 weeks, neurological disorders, pregnancy, chronic infectious disease or cancer, mania or substance use disorder, and use of antipsychotics, mood stabilizers, or systemic steroids. Study procedures were approved by the University of Wisconsin-Madison Health Sciences IRB. All participants provided written informed consent.

The pandemic halted in-person data collection in March 2020 for the larger NIH-funded project from which the current investigation is drawn. When in-person data collection resumed in February 2021, changes were made to mitigate the risk of infection, including changes to TSST procedures. Therefore, the larger project now includes a Cohort 1 (with 77 participants enrolled prior to March 2020) and a Cohort 2 (ongoing). The analyses presented herein include data from Cohort 1. Hypotheses and the data analysis plan were pre-registered on the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/t8r3w) prior to analysis.

Data collection

Trier Social Stress Test

The Trier Social Stress Test (TSST), a social evaluative stressor (Kirschbaum et al., Citation1993), was administered during a laboratory visit at approximately 2:00 pm and consisted of a 5-min preparatory period, 5-min speech, and 5-min mental arithmetic test spoken into a microphone in front of a panel of judges and video camera. See for a schematic of the TSST and measures.

Figure 1. Schematic of TSST and measures. The TSST (Kirschbaum et al., Citation1993) consisted of 5 min of anticipation, 5 min speech, and 5 min mental math periods. The current report addresses salivary cortisol taken at baseline, immediately post-TSST, and 10 min post-TSST. The PANAS-now (Watson et al., Citation1988) was administered immediately pre-TSST and immediately post-TSST. The question “How stressed are you now?” was assessed at baseline and immediately post-TSST. No self-report measures were administered at 10 min post-TSST. Mean cortisol levels at each time point are depicted.

Concise listing of experimental procedures with a line graph showing the increase in cortisol levels from baseline to after the stressor task.
Figure 1. Schematic of TSST and measures. The TSST (Kirschbaum et al., Citation1993) consisted of 5 min of anticipation, 5 min speech, and 5 min mental math periods. The current report addresses salivary cortisol taken at baseline, immediately post-TSST, and 10 min post-TSST. The PANAS-now (Watson et al., Citation1988) was administered immediately pre-TSST and immediately post-TSST. The question “How stressed are you now?” was assessed at baseline and immediately post-TSST. No self-report measures were administered at 10 min post-TSST. Mean cortisol levels at each time point are depicted.

We measured state affect prior to the preparatory period and following mental arithmetic using the state version of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS-now) (Watson et al., Citation1988). Participants also rated self-reported stress on a 1–10 scale at baseline and immediately following the TSST. See for self-report means.

Table 1. Cortisol levels, affect, and self-reported stress with respect to the TSST.

Cortisol response was assessed using saliva from Salivette® cortisol tubes (Sarstedt, Nümbrecht, Germany). We were interested in initial cortisol reactivity to the stressor and therefore focused on samples acquired immediately post-TSST and 10 min post-TSST with respect to baseline levels. Additional samples were acquired during the recovery period following the TSST and will be used in the future to examine cortisol stress recovery as an aspect of the larger project. See and for mean cortisol levels for time points included in analyses reported here.

Participant characteristics

Prior studies have shown that the relationship between cortisol and psychological variables differs as a function of participant characteristics (Soravia et al., Citation2006, Citation2009;  Gaffey et al. 2019). For the current project, we assessed depression symptoms using the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) (Radloff, Citation1977), anxiety symptoms using the trait anxiety scale of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) (Spielberger et al., Citation1970), and age on the day of the TSST. The investigation is not designed to test differences based on gender or race. However, we created binary variables of self-endorsed sex assigned at birth (female; not female) and race (self-endorsed as Black, Indigenous, and/or Asian; self-endorsed as White) to confirm that primary findings remained significant when including either of these in the model.

Data processing

Emotional response to the TSST

Our pre-registration (https://osf.io/t8r3w) identified the PANAS-now negative affect (NA) scale (Watson et al., Citation1988) as our primary index of emotional response to the TSST. We used the difference of post-TSST NA minus pre-TSST NA to index change in NA. Our pre-registration identified two secondary psychological measures, PANAS-now positive affect (PA) (Watson et al., Citation1988) and self-reported stress. As with NA, we use the post-TSST minus pre-TSST differences as indices of change in PA and self-reported stress.

Salivary cortisol processing

Cortisol samples were stored at −80 °C until analysis. Salivary cortisol was measured using an ElectroChemiLuminescence immunoassay (ECLIA; Roche, Basel, Switzerland). The inter-assay and intra-assay coefficients of variation were 13.2% and 10.3%, respectively. Five participants were unable or unwilling to provide saliva samples at baseline or after the stressor. An additional three participants had a saliva sample of insufficient volume for the assay. We removed data from one outlier, who had a cortisol response at 10 min post-TSST greater than our pre-registered criteria for an outlier of three standard deviations from the mean, resulting in a total sample of 68 participants with sufficient cortisol data for analysis. The distributions of the cortisol data were approximately normal. Transformations were not applied because they over-corrected and created negative skew.

To index the cortisol response to the TSST, we computed area under the curve with respect to increase (AUCi) (Pruessner et al., Citation2003). Four participants were missing valid cortisol data from the immediate post-TSST (one participant) or the 10 min post-TSST sample (three participants). For these participants, we imputed values from the two neighboring sample timepoints using linear interpolation, which was preferrable to multiple imputation given the rapidly changing cortisol levels in the TSST (Graham, Citation2009). Using this method, we retained 68 participants for analysis. We were primarily interested in cortisol increase from baseline to the sample when self-report was assessed concurrently, i.e. immediately post-TSST. This AUCi value is perfectly correlated with and therefore mathematically identical to the difference of post-TSST minus baseline cortisol levels.

Data analysis

Analyses were conducted using SAS Enterprise Guide version 8.3 update 2. As described above, we restricted analysis to cortisol samples at baseline, immediately post-TSST, and 10 min post-TSST to address the relationship between cortisol and affective reactivity to acute stress. Using general linear modeling analysis techniques, we tested the relationship between emotional response and cortisol AUCi. We conducted post-hoc multiple regression with the three cortisol time points (baseline, post-TSST, 10 min post-TSST) as simultaneous predictors of emotional response to clarify the magnitude and direction of the relationship between cortisol level and emotional response at each time point, while accounting for baseline levels. For multiple regression analyses, we corrected for multiple comparisons (i.e. the inclusion of multiple unique cortisol time points) using an alpha level of 0.005 required to meet significance. We also explored whether results held after adjusting for variance related to age, depression, or anxiety severity. In addition, we tested whether results held when sex assigned at birth or race were included in the model.

Results

Salivary cortisol, negative affect, and self-reported stress increased from baseline to post-TSST, ps < 0.001, but there was no change in positive affect (see ). Increase in NA was inversely related to cortisol AUCi from baseline to post-TSST, r(67) = −0.24, p = 0.049 (see ). However, the increase in cortisol through 10 min post-TSST (i.e. AUCi including baseline, post-TSST, and 10 min post-TSST) was not significantly related to change in NA, p = 0.12. Post-hoc multiple regression analysis, including baseline cortisol in the model, also demonstrated that cortisol levels immediately post-TSST were significantly inversely related to increase in NA (see ). However, cortisol levels at 10 min post-TSST were positively related to increase in NA but not significantly so after correction for multiple comparisons (see ). Inclusion of age, depression severity, or anxiety severity in the model did not change the pattern of results (see Supplemental Material). When sex was included in the model, the significant inverse relationship between AUCi (from baseline to post-TSST) and increase in NA remained, F(3,64) = 4.14, p = 0.046. When race was included in the model, the inverse relationship between AUCi (from baseline to post-TSST) and increase in NA remained but was no longer significant, F(3,64) = 3.91, p = 0.052.

Figure 2. Greater stress-induced cortisol increases are associated with smaller increases in negative affect during stress. Specifically, increase in cortisol (AUCi; original units in nmol/L) is inversely correlated with increase in negative affect assessed using the PANAS immediately after the TSST with respect to baseline, r(67) = −0.24, p < 0.049.

Scatter plot of the inverse relationship between the increase in cortisol and the increase in negative affect.
Figure 2. Greater stress-induced cortisol increases are associated with smaller increases in negative affect during stress. Specifically, increase in cortisol (AUCi; original units in nmol/L) is inversely correlated with increase in negative affect assessed using the PANAS immediately after the TSST with respect to baseline, r(67) = −0.24, p < 0.049.

Table 2. Multiple regression predicting increase in negative affect (NA) in response to the TSST.

Change in PA was not related to TSST-evoked cortisol. The relationship between increase in cortisol measured immediately post-TSST and increase in self-reported stress level was in the same direction as findings for NA (i.e. an inverse relation), but was not significant, r(65) = −0.22, p = 0.07.

Discussion

We found that negative emotional reactivity was inversely related to endogenous cortisol reactivity measured immediately after the TSST. This finding supported our hypothesis that greater stress-induced acute cortisol increases are associated with smaller increases in negative affect. This is consistent with prior research investigating acute stress suggestive of mood-protective effects of cortisol elevations (Het et al., Citation2012; Nakataki et al., Citation2017; Soravia et al., Citation2006).

In the current study, the inverse relationship between cortisol and NA was apparent for cortisol measured concurrently with NA, immediately after the TSST. The timing of these findings differs from Het et al.’s prior research demonstrating an inverse relation between negative affect and cortisol elevation at 10 min after a stressor (Het et al., Citation2012). Unfortunately, we did not assess NA at 10 min after the stressor and therefore additional research is needed to fully replicate Het et al. (Citation2012) Future research should address whether the relationship between affect and cortisol depends on timing of measurement with respect to a stressor. This is particularly important because prior research demonstrates that neural and molecular effects of acute stress depend on timing (Joëls, Citation2018). For instance, Joëls has demonstrated time-varying effects of stressors on neural function in humans and rodents, which are related to rapid non-genomic effects at corticosteroid receptors (largely mineralocorticoid receptor-dependent) and delayed genomic effects (largely glucocorticoid receptor-dependent) (Joëls, Citation2018). In addition, research in humans demonstrates that timing and affective chronometry are key. For example, Hellhammer and Schubert showed that perceived stress measured during but not after stress was positively related to the magnitude of cortisol response (Hellhammer & Schubert, Citation2012). Thus, while the current project extends prior research suggestive of an inverse association between heightened cortisol and negative affective response measured immediately after a stressor (Het et al., Citation2012), future research should address whether this association varies depending on the timing of responses with respect to the onset, duration, and cessation of an acute stressor.

The mounting evidence demonstrating mood-buffering effects of stress-evoked cortisol elevations (Het et al., Citation2012; Nakataki et al., Citation2017; Soravia et al., Citation2006) opposes popular conceptions of cortisol as associated with or even causing negative affective responses to stress (McEwen, Citation2019). Multiple studies now demonstrate that emotional and cortisol reactivity to acute stress are more dissociable than is often assumed (Het et al., Citation2012; Nakataki et al., Citation2017; Soravia et al., Citation2006). This more nuanced picture of possible mood-buffering effects of cortisol responses to acute stress has important ramifications for “stress reduction” and emotion regulation practices, especially in the context of acute stressors. Cortisol reactivity to a stressor may promote adaptive emotion regulation, which is consistent with prior research demonstrating adaptive neural and cognitive effects of transient cortisol elevations (Gaffey et al., Citation2019; McEwen, Citation2019). These findings are in line with theoretical models positing that robust yet transient physiological reactivity is adaptive within the context of emotionally provocative events (Schaefer et al., Citation2018). The biological and psychological aspects of stress reduction are clearly more complex than a simple dampening of stress physiology (Het et al., Citation2012; McEwen, Citation2019).

The relation between cortisol and affective function is relevant for mental health conditions (Gaffey et al., Citation2019; Yehuda et al., Citation2009). While the relation between immediate responses to the TSST and longer-term changes in affect is unknown, psychological benefits of transient cortisol elevation have been demonstrated in a variety of clinical contexts and conditions (Gaffey et al., Citation2019; Galatzer-Levy et al., Citation2014). Intervention focused on transiently enhancing the cortisol signal is an important area for future clinical investigation (Gaffey et al., Citation2019). Emotional aspects of “stress reduction” may at times be supported with transient elevations in cortisol, consistent with mood-buffering effects of interventions involving “eustress” such as exercise (Rebar et al., Citation2015).

Limitations and future directions

The project from which the current analyses were drawn was hindered by the COVID-19 pandemic, which brought in-person data collection to a halt. The current report addresses findings for the participants brought into the laboratory prior to the pandemic-related national emergency, and analyses are restricted to TSST reactivity. We are retaining cortisol samples later in the TSST recovery period for future analyses because the time course of emotional recovery is a key aspect of the larger project. The goal of the current project is a limited analysis and a focused replication test of prior TSST cortisol and NA reactivity findings (Het et al., Citation2012). Though this analysis is limited, we believe it is important to publish pre-registered replications of prior research and to debunk misconceptions about the relationship between cortisol and emotional reactivity to acute stress.

The current analyses are limited in that we address only cortisol and not autonomic nervous system reactivity to stress, which has been shown to be buffered by exogenous cortisol in one study (Soravia et al., Citation2006), but to correlate positively with NA after stress in another (Het et al., Citation2012). The complex inter-relations among cortisol, autonomic, and affective responses to stress is an exciting area for future research.

Prior research has demonstrated that relationships between cortisol elevations and psychological processes vary as a function of individual differences in personal characteristics and psychopathology (Gaffey et al., Citation2019; Soravia et al., Citation2006, Citation2009). We demonstrated that results largely held when including several relevant variables in the model, including depression symptoms, anxiety symptoms, age, sex assigned at birth, and race. However, we lacked the power to adequately test whether these personal characteristics moderate relationships between cortisol and affective reactivity to stress. The current report should not be taken as evidence of a lack of such moderation, which should be addressed in future research.

Conclusion

As hypothesized in our pre-registration, we found that greater stress-induced cortisol increases are associated with smaller increases in negative affect in response to acute stress. The results partially replicate findings of Het et al. (Het et al., Citation2012), though the timing of results differs somewhat and should be addressed in future research. The findings add to a growing body of evidence suggestive of mood-protective effects of acute cortisol elevations, and demonstrate that emotional and cortisol reactivity to acute stress are more dissociable than is often assumed (Het et al., Citation2012; Nakataki et al., Citation2017; Soravia et al., Citation2006). These findings may have important mental health implications and suggest that investigation of behavioral and/or pharmacological interventions aimed at transiently enhancing the cortisol signal is an exciting area for future research.

Author contributions

SMS, MAR, RJD are key personnel and designed the R01-funded project from which this investigation is drawn, including methods and procedures related to the TSST. ALB, ECN, & SMS collected and processed data. SMS, AJF, ETH, MAR, DWG, & HCA planned analyses for TSST data. HCA analyzed the data, with assistance from DWG, SMS, & ALB. HCA wrote the first draft and all authors provided input on the manuscript.

Supplemental material

Supplemental Material

Download MS Word (14.5 KB)

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank the Emotion and Wellness study participants, the Center for Healthy Minds Research Support Core, the Survey of the Health of Wisconsin for assistance in recruitment, and the Wisconsin National Primate Research Center’s Assay Services for salivary cortisol processing (NIH P51OD011106). We would also like to thank current and former research staff and TSST judges, including Leonard Black, Gabriela Marulanda, Lauren Gresham, Jeanne Harris, and Corrina Frye.

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).

Data availability statement

Data are shared and available through the National Institute of Mental Health Data Archive.

Additional information

Funding

This work is supported by the National Institute of Mental Health [R01MH043454, Davidson and Schaefer]. In addition, Dr. Grupe is supported by a National Institute of Mental Health career development award [K01MH117222]. Dr. Richard J. Davidson is the founder, president, and serves on the board of directors for the nonprofit organization, Healthy Minds Innovations, Inc. No other authors have any conflicts of interest or competing interest.

Notes on contributors

Heather C. Abercrombie

Heather Abercrombie is a Scientist and Licensed Psychologist at the UW-Madison Center for Healthy Minds. Her research has focused primarily on the relationship between acute cortisol elevations and psychological function in depressed and non-depressed humans.

Alexandra L. Barnes

Alexandra Barnes is an Associate Research Specialist at the UW-Madison Center for Healthy Minds.

Elizabeth C. Nord

Elizabeth Nord is a Research Specialist at the UW-Madison Center for Healthy Minds.

Anna J. Finley

Anna Finley is a Postdoctoral Scholar at the UW-Madison Center for Healthy Minds.

Estelle T. Higgins

Estelle Higgins is a Graduate Student in the UW-Madison Department of Psychology.

Daniel W. Grupe

Daniel Grupe is a Scientist at the UW-Madison Center for Healthy Minds.

Melissa A. Rosenkranz

Melissa Rosenkranz is Core Faculty at the Center for Healthy Minds, Distinguished Chair in Contemplative Neuroscience, and an Assistant Professor of Psychiatry at UW-Madison.

Richard J. Davidson

Richard Davidson is the William James and Vilas Professor of Psychology and Psychiatry, Founder and Director of the UW-Madison Center for Healthy Minds, and Founder of Healthy Minds Innovations. He is also co-PI on the NIH grant that funded this research.

Stacey M. Schaefer

Stacey Schaefer is a Scientist at UW-Madison and Co-Leader on the MIDUS Neuroscience Project. She is also Co-PI on the NIH grant that funded this research.

References

  • Gaffey, A. E., Walsh, E. C., Ladd, C. O., Hoks, R. M., & Abercrombie, H. C. (2019). Alterations in systemic and cognitive glucocorticoid sensitivity in depression. Biological Psychiatry. Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 4(3), 310–320. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2018.11.007
  • Galatzer-Levy, I. R., Steenkamp, M. M., Brown, A. D., Qian, M., Inslicht, S., Henn-Haase, C., Otte, C., Yehuda, R., Neylan, T. C., & Marmar, C. R. (2014). Cortisol response to an experimental stress paradigm prospectively predicts long-term distress and resilience trajectories in response to active police service. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 56, 36–42. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.04.020
  • Graham, J. W. (2009). Missing data analysis: Making it work in the real world. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 549–576. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085530
  • Hellhammer, J., & Schubert, M. (2012). The physiological response to Trier Social Stress Test relates to subjective measures of stress during but not before or after the test. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 37(1), 119–124. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.05.012
  • Het, S., & Wolf, O. T. (2007). Mood changes in response to psychosocial stress in healthy young women: Effects of pretreatment with cortisol. Behavioral neuroscience, 121(1), 11–20. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.121.1.11
  • Het, S., Schoofs, D., Rohleder, N., & Wolf, O. T. (2012). Stress-induced cortisol level elevations are associated with reduced negative affect after stress: Indications for a mood-buffering cortisol effect. Psychosomatic Medicine, 74(1), 23–32. https://doi.org/10.1097/PSY.0b013e31823a4a25
  • Joëls, M. (2018). Corticosteroids and the brain. Journal of Endocrinology, 238(3), 121-130. https://doi.org/10.1530/JOE-18-0226
  • Kirschbaum, C., & Hellhammer, D. H. (1994). Salivary cortisol in psychoneuroendocrine research: Recent developments and applications. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 19(4), 313–333. https://doi.org/10.1016/0306-4530(94)90013-2
  • Kirschbaum, C., Pirke, K. M., & Hellhammer, D. H. (1993). The ‘Trier Social Stress Test’–a tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology, 28(1–2), 76–81. https://doi.org/10.1159/000119004
  • McEwen, B. S. (2019). What is the confusion with cortisol? Chronic Stress, 3, 247054701983347. https://doi.org/10.1177/2470547019833647
  • Nakataki, M., Soravia, L. M., Schwab, S., Horn, H., Dierks, T., Strik, W., Wiest, R., Heinrichs, M., de Quervain, D. J.-F., Federspiel, A., & Morishima, Y. (2017). Glucocorticoid administration improves aberrant fear-processing networks in spider phobia. Neuropsychopharmacology : Official Publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology, 42(2), 485–494. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2016.207
  • Pruessner, J. C., Kirschbaum, C., Meinlschmid, G., & Hellhammer, D. H. (2003). Two formulas for computation of the area under the curve represent measures of total hormone concentration versus time-dependent change. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 28(7), 916–931. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0306-4530(02)00108-7
  • Radloff, L. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1(3), 385–401. https://doi.org/10.1177/014662167700100306
  • Rebar, A. L., Stanton, R., Geard, D., Short, C., Duncan, M. J., & Vandelanotte, C. (2015). A meta-meta-analysis of the effect of physical activity on depression and anxiety in non-clinical adult populations. Health Psychology Review, 9(3), 366–378. https://doi.org/10.1080/17437199.2015.1022901
  • Schaefer, S. M., van Reekum, C. M., Lapate, R. C., Heller, A. S., Grupe, D. W., & Davidson, R. J. (2018). The temporal dynamics of emotional responding: Implications for well-being and health from the MIDUS neuroscience project. In C. D. Ryff & R. F. Krueger (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of integrative health science (pp. 354–366). Oxford University Press.
  • Soravia, L. M., de Quervain, D. J. F., & Heinrichs, M. (2009). Glucocorticoids do not reduce subjective fear in healthy subjects exposed to social stress. Biological Psychology, 81(3), 184–188. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.04.001
  • Soravia, L. M., Heinrichs, M., Aerni, A., Maroni, C., Schelling, G., Ehlert, U., Roozendaal, B., & de Quervain, D. J.-F. (2006). Glucocorticoids reduce phobic fear in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103(14), 5585–5590. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0509184103
  • Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., & Lushene, R. E. (1970). STAI manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory. Consulting Psychologists Press, Inc.
  • Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1063–1070. [Database] https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063
  • Yehuda, R., Cai, G., Golier, J. A., Sarapas, C., Galea, S., Ising, M., Rein, T., Schmeidler, J., Müller-Myhsok, B., Holsboer, F., & Buxbaum, J. D. (2009). Gene expression patterns associated with posttraumatic stress disorder following exposure to the World Trade Center attacks. Biological psychiatry, 66(7), 708–711. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.02.034