914
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

The blind obedience of others: a better than average effect in a Milgram-like experiment

&

References

  • Alicke, M. D., & Govorun, O. (2005). The better-than-average effect. In M. D. Alicke, D. A. Dunning, & J. I. Krueger (Eds.), Studies in self and identity (pp. 85–106). Psychology Press.
  • Banyard, V. L., Moynihan, M. M., & Crossman, M. T. (2009). Reducing sexual violence on campus: The role of student leaders as empowered bystanders. Journal of College Student Development, 50(4), 446–457. https://doi.org/10.1353/csd.0.0083
  • Bègue, L., Beauvois, J. -L., Courbet, D., Oberlé, D., Lepage, J., & Duke, A. A. (2015). Personality predicts obedience in a Milgram paradigm. Journal of Personality, 83(3), 299–306. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12104
  • Bègue, L., & Vezirian, K. (2022a). Instrumental harm toward animals in a Milgram-Like experiment: The role of non-pathological personality traits. In O. Chan & R. Wong (Eds.), Animal abuse and interpersonal violence: A psycho-criminological understanding. Wiley.
  • Bègue, L., & Vezirian, K. (2022b). Sacrificing animals in the name of scientific authority: The relationship between pro-scientific mindset and the lethal use of animals in biomedical experimentation. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 48(10), 1483–1498. https://doi.org/10.1177/01461672211039413
  • Blass, T. (1991). Understanding behavior in the Milgram obedience experiment: The role of personality, situations, and their interactions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60(3), 398–413. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.60.3.398
  • Brown, J. D. (2012). Understanding the better than average effect: Motives (still) matter. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 38(2), 209–219. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167211432763
  • Burger, J. M. (2009). Replicating Milgram: Would people still obey today? The American Psychologist, 64(1), 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0010932
  • Caspar, E. A., Christensen, J. F., Cleeremans, A., & Haggard, P. (2016). Coercion changes the sense of agency in the human brain. Current Biology, 26(5), 585–592. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.12.067
  • Chambers, J. R., Windschitl, P. D., & Suls, J. (2003). Egocentrism, event frequency, and comparative optimism: When what happens frequently is “more likely to happen to me”. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 29(11), 1343–1356. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167203256870
  • Codol, J. P. (1975). On the so-called ‘superior conformity of the self’ behavior: Twenty experimental investigations. European Journal of Social Psychology, 5(4), 457–501. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2420050404
  • Darley, J. M., & Latané, B. (1968). Bystander intervention in emergencies: Diffusion of responsibility. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 8(4, Pt.1), 377–383. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0025589
  • Doliński, D., Grzyb, T., Folwarczny, M., Grzybała, P., Krzyszycha, K., Martynowska, K., & Trojanowski, J. (2017). Would you deliver an electric shock in 2015? Obedience in the experimental paradigm developed by Stanley Milgram in the 50 years following the original studies. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 8(8), 1–7. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550617693060
  • Duck, J. M., & Mullin, B. -A. (1995). The perceived impact of the mass media: Reconsidering the third person effect. European Journal of Social Psychology, 25(1), 77–93. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2420250107
  • Duck, J. M., Terry, D. J., & Hogg, M. A. (1995). The perceived influence of AIDS advertising: Third-person effects in the context of positive media content. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 17(3), 305–325. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15324834basp1703_2
  • Elms, A., & Milgram, S. (1966). Personality characteristics associated with obedience and defiance toward authoritative commands. Journal of Experimental Research in Personality, 1(4), 289–292.
  • Epley, N., & Dunning, D. (2000). Feeling “holier than thou”: Are self-serving assessments produced by errors in self- or social prediction? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(6), 861–875. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.79.6.861
  • Farley, S. D., Carson, D., & Pope, T. J. (2019). “I would never fall for that”: The use of an illegitimate authority to teach social psychological principles. Teaching of Psychology, 46(2), 146–152. https://doi.org/10.1177/0098628319834200
  • Foster, C. A., Clarke, J. A., & Packard, G. A. (2018). Can I see the real me? Leadership ability and the better-than-average effect. Military Psychology, 30(5), 390–397. https://doi.org/10.1080/08995605.2018.1478545
  • Gibson, S. (2013). Milgram’s obedience experiments: A rhetorical analysis. British Journal of Social Psychology, 52(2), 290–309. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8309.2011.02070.x
  • Graupmann, V., & Frey, D. (2014). Bad examples: How thinking about blind obedience can induce responsibility and courage. Peace and Conflict: Journal of Peace Psychology, 20(2), 124–134.
  • Greenwald, A. G. (1980). The totalitarian ego: Fabrication and revision of personal history. The American Psychologist, 35(7), 603–618. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.35.7.603
  • Grzyb, T., & Dolinski, D. (2017). Beliefs about Obedience Levels in Studies Conducted within the Milgram Paradigm: Better than Average Effect and Comparisons of Typical Behaviors by Residents of Various Nations. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1632.
  • Haslam, S. A., & Reicher, S. D. (2017). 50 years of “Obedience to authority”: From blind conformity to engaged followership. Annual Review of Law and Social Science, 13(1), 59–78. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-lawsocsci-110316-113710
  • Haslam, S. A., Reicher, S. D., Millard, K., & McDonald, R. (2015). ‘Happy to have been of service’: The Yale archive as a window into the engaged followership of participants in Milgram’s ‘obedience’ experiments. British Journal of Social Psychology, 54(1), 55–83. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12074
  • Kim, Y. H., Kwon, H., & Chiu, C. Y. (2017). The better-than-average effect is observed because “Average” is often construed as below-median ability. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 898. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00898
  • Klein, N., & Epley, N. (2017). Less evil than you: Bounded self-righteousness in character inferences, emotional reactions, and behavioral extremes. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 43(8), 1202–1212. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167217711918
  • Langer, E. J. (1975). The illusion of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32(2), 311–328. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.32.2.311
  • Leviston, Z., & Uren, H. V. (2020). Overestimating one’s “Green” behavior: Better-than-average bias may function to reduce perceived personal threat from climate change. The Journal of Social Issues, 76(1), 70–85. https://doi.org/10.1111/josi.12365
  • Logg, J. M., Haran, U., & Moore, D. A. (2018). Is overconfidence a motivated bias? Experimental evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 147(10), 1445–1465. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000500
  • Loughnan, S., Leidner, B., Doron, G., Haslam, N., Kashima, Y., Tong, J., & Yeung, V. (2010). Universal biases in self-perception: Better and more human than average. British Journal of Social Psychology, 49(3), 627–636. https://doi.org/10.1348/014466610X487779
  • Lowery, B. S., Hardin, C. D., & Sinclair, S. (2001). Social Influence Effects on Automatic Racial Prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 842–855.
  • Milgram, S. (1963). Behavioral Study of obedience. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 67(4), 371–378. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0040525
  • Milgram, S. (1965). Some conditions of obedience and disobedience to authority. Human Relations, 18(1), 57–76. https://doi.org/10.1177/001872676501800105
  • Milgram, S. (1974). Obedience to authority: An experimental view. Harper & Row.
  • Mojzisch, A., Elster, C., & Germar, M. (2022). People perceive themselves to adhere more strictly to COVID-19 guidelines than others. Psychology, Health & Medicine, 27(2), 325–332. https://doi.org/10.1080/13548506.2021.1906435
  • Mujal, G. N., Taylor, M. E., Fry, J. L., Gochez-Kerr, T. H., & Weaver, N. L. (2021). A systematic review of bystander interventions for the prevention of sexual violence. Trauma, Violence, & Abuse, 22(2), 381–396. https://doi.org/10.1177/1524838019849587
  • Otten, W., & van der Pligt, J. (1996). Context effects in the measurement of optimism in probability judgment. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 15(1), 80–101. https://doi.org/10.1521/jscp.1996.15.1.80
  • Perloff, R. M. (1999). The third-person effect: A critical review and synthesis. Media Psychology, 1(4), 353–378. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532785xmep0104_4
  • Perry, G. (2012). Behind the shock machine: The untold story of the notorious Milgram psychology experiments. Scribe Publications.
  • Reicher, S., & Haslam, S. A. (2011). After shock? Towards a social identity explanation of the Milgram ‘obedience’ studies: Social identity and the Milgram studies. British Journal of Social Psychology, 50(1), 163–169. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8309.2010.02015.x
  • Reicher, S., Haslam, A., & Smith, J. (2012). Working toward the experimenter: Reconceptualizing obedience within the Milgram paradigm as identification-based followership. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(4), 315–324. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691612448482
  • Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and adolescent self-image. Princeton University Press.
  • Sedikides, C., Meek, R., Alicke, M. D., & Taylor, S. (2014). Behind bars but above the bar: Prisoners consider themselves more prosocial than non-prisoners. British Journal of Social Psychology, 53(2), 396–403. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12060
  • Steffens, N. K., Haslam, S. A., Jetten, Y., & Mols, F. (2018). Our followers are lions, theirs are sheep: How social identity shapes theories about the nature of followership and the mechanisms of social influence. Political Psychology, 39(1), 23–42. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12387
  • Suls, J., Lemos, K., & Stewart, H. L. (2002). Self-esteem, construal, and comparisons with the self, friends, and peers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(2), 252–261. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.82.2.252
  • Violato, E., King, S., & Bulut, O. (2022). Conformity, obedience, and the Better than Average Effect in health professional students. Canadian Medical Education Journal, 13(1), 55–64. https://doi.org/10.36834/cmej.71970
  • Woodzicka, J. A., & LaFrance, M. (2001). Real versus imagined gender harassment. The Journal of Social Issues, 57(1), 15–30. https://doi.org/10.1111/0022-4537.00199
  • Zdunek, R. Z., Czarna, A. Z., & Sedikides, C. (2022). Grandiose (communal and agentic) narcissism and predicted disobedience in the Milgram paradigm. Personality and Individual Differences, 189, 11154. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2022.111514
  • Zell, E., Strickhouser, J. E., Sedikides, C., & Alicke, M. D. (2020). The better-than-average effect in comparative self-evaluation: A comprehensive review and meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 146(2), 118–149.
  • Ziano, I., Mok, P. Y., & Feldman, G. (2021). Replication and Extension of Alicke (1985) Better-Than-Average Effect for Desirable and Controllable Traits. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 12(6), 1005–1017.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.