630
Views
5
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Communication in Primary Healthcare: A State-of-the-Art Literature Review of Conversation-Analytic Research

ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon

ABSTRACT

We report the first state-of-the-art review of conversation-analytic (CA) research on communication in primary healthcare. We conducted a systematic search across multiple bibliographic databases and specialist sources and employed backward and forward citation tracking. We included 177 empirical studies spanning four decades of research and 16 different countries/health systems, with data in 17 languages. The majority of studies originated in United States and United Kingdom and focused on medical visits between physicians and adult patients. We generated three broad research themes in order to synthesize the study findings: managing agendas, managing participation, and managing authority. We characterize the state-of-the-art for each theme, illustrating the progression of the work and making comparisons across different languages and health systems, where possible. We consider practical applications of the findings, reflect on the state of current knowledge, and suggest some directions for future research. Data reported are in multiple languages.

Overview and background

Primary healthcare (PHC) is a key element of health systems in many countries, distinct from other aspects of health services delivery systems, such as specialty outpatient care (see Ekberg et al., Citation2024) or emergency medical care (see Riou, Citation2024). PHC is widely recognized as the “front door” into health care systems, providing ambulatory or first-contact personal healthcare services, and often performing a gate-keeping role. It is rooted ostensibly in a commitment to social justice, equity, and participation:

PHC is a whole-of-society approach to health that aims at ensuring the highest possible level of health and well-being and their equitable distribution by focusing on people’s needs and as early as possible along the continuum from health promotion and disease prevention to treatment, rehabilitation and palliative care, and as close as feasible to people’s everyday environment. (WHO & UNICEF, Citation2018, p. xii)

In the United Kingdom, for example, PHC involves treatment for acute minor illnesses, and the management of long-term conditions such as diabetes and heart disease, as well as the prevention of potential future ill-health through health promotion, immunization, and screening programmes.

Comprehensive generalist physicians (i.e., general practitioners or family physicians) and nurses play a central role as trained specialists in PHC. In some healthcare systems, other medical specialists (e.g., internists specializing in adult internal medicine and pediatricians) play a key role. Primary care teams may also include pharmacists, paramedics, physiotherapists, occupational therapists, midwives, physician assistants/associates, community health workers, and dentists.

In this review, we focus on conversation-analytic (CA) studies of the provision of treatment for acute minor illnesses and the management of long-term conditions. Our rationale is purely pragmatic: A review of all CA studies meeting the wide remit of PHC as described above would be difficult to contain. Moreover, such heterogeneity would reduce the possibility of identifying systematic and differential practices of action, and of making useful comparisons across countries and languages (see inclusion and exclusion criteria). Within these parameters, the aim of our review was to perform a comprehensive search of the historical and current literature. In what follows we describe our methods and present a narrative synthesis of the findings. We then consider practical applications, reflect on our approach to the review and the strengths and weaknesses of the field to-date, and suggest some priorities for future research.

Methods

Our first step was a scoping search that included existing or ongoing reviews of CA in PHC. After reviewing the coverage, relevance, and accuracy of the search findings with an information specialist, we decided on the following search terms: “conversation analysis,” “medicine,” and “primary health care.” We ran this search across the following bibliographic databases: Cinahl, Embase, Medline, PsychINFO, SSCI, and Scopus. No date restrictions were employed. To ensure the inclusion of all relevant material (e.g., book chapters), we consulted the EM/CA wikiFootnote1 bibliography of medical CA studies and searched our personal collections, online contents of popular journals in the field, and checked the publication pages of key scholars. The final search date was March 11, 2022.

All results were exported into Rayyan,Footnote2 a collaborative review software package, and any duplicates removed. Next, titles and abstracts were screened independently by both authors against the following inclusion and exclusion criteria.

Inclusion criteria

  1. CA studies of communication in PHC (including face-to-face and telephone consultations, new and follow-up acute care appointments, routine chronic care appointments, annual checkups, new patient appointments, and urgent primary care);

  2. published journal articles (including pre-prints where available), book chapters, and official reports;

  3. all countries of origin; and

  4. all languages where final publication is in English.

Exclusion criteria

  1. studies of preventative care (including vaccinations), physical rehabilitation, or palliative care;

  2. studies of wider PHC (e.g., maternity services, community mental health services, community pharmacy, physiotherapy services, dentistry, audiology, and optometry);

  3. studies of manipulated PHC practice (i.e., simulated data or data from primary care trials);

  4. studies of interactions with trainees (e.g., medical students) or support staff (e.g., interpreters, receptionists);

  5. studies comparing PHC data with data from other healthcare settings;

  6. monographs, literature reviews, commentaries, editorials, and conference abstracts; and

  7. masters’ dissertations or Ph.D. theses.

Any conflicts were highlighted in Rayyan and resolved during regular meetings between the two authors. Full texts were retrieved for the remaining studies. When articles or book chapters were unavailable online, we sought copies from corresponding authors or requested interlibrary loans. All full texts were listed alphabetically and then split equally between the authors for closer screening against our criteria. All excluded texts were labeled in Rayyan with reasons for exclusion and any uncertainties flagged as “maybe” for subsequent discussion (e.g., if we were unsure if the context counted as PHC). Once we had agreed on our final included studies, we used Google Scholar to conduct forward and backward citation tracking to identify any further studies for screening. On completion, 177 studies were included for analysis.

We developed an Excel table to organize data extraction. For the purposes of this review, we did not assess the quality of our included studies. Where evident, studies using the same dataset were highlighted, and missing information was sought from other sources. We tabulated studies chronologically to track development of the field over time. The study findings were imported into NVivo. Codes were created, applied, and refined, and from these we generated three themes. Our decision -making here was informed by (1) the desire to bring together systematic and differential practices of action identified and (2) a wish to contextualize the findings, where possible, with topics in the wider PHC literature and/or professional guidance.

Current state-of-the-art

A total of 177 CA studies of PHC published between 1981 and 2022 were included. Of these, 137 were peer-reviewed research articles (90 from medical or health-related journals), and 40 were chapters in edited books. The studies originated from 16 countries, dominated by the United States (n = 69) and the United Kingdom (n = 52), with data in 17 different languages (see ).

Table 1. Included studies.

The overarching and interrelated broad themes generated were managing agendas, managing participation, and managing authority. Below, we describe each theme, provide illustrative examples, and highlight both similar and, where appropriate, contrasting findings across countries and languages. We also attend to chronology, to allow insight into trends, developments, and trajectories in the field over time.

Managing agendas

The acute primary care consultation is structurally organized to address a singular “chief” problem or concern; however, patients often present to PHC with multiple agenda items (Leydon et al., Citation2018). Managing or negotiating agendas is a perennial problem for physicians and patients (Manning & Ray, Citation2002). If done successfully, it can avoid the potential for “unvoiced” agenda items and maximize efficiency (Barry et al., Citation2000). In the PHC literature and professional guidance, agenda setting (i.e., soliciting the patient’s agenda for the consultation to reconcile with the physician’s own agenda; see Levenstein et al., Citation1986), has been championed as a strategy for addressing this problem. It provides an organizing framework for physicians to prioritize which issues can be dealt with, and which should be deferred to another visit or referred elsewhere.

Physicians’ agenda-setting practices

Examining the practices whereby physicians manage agenda setting has been and continues to be a key research theme in CA studies of PHC. One such practice is to elicit the patient’s chief concern at the outset of the consultation. Indeed, this was the focus of the earliest CA study included in our review (Heath, Citation1981). Drawing on British data collected throughout the 1970s, Heath characterized the different formats of physicians’ “first topic initiators” or opening questions that solicit the patient’s reason for the visit (Heath, Citation1981). Distinguishing between “Type A” first topic initiators found in new appointments (e.g., “How can I help?”), and “Type B,” found in return appointments (e.g., “How are you doing?”), Heath demonstrated how physicians’ opening questions were designed for the recipient, providing for the generation of an “appropriate and correct” first topic or “disclosure sequence” (Heath, Citation1981, p. 78) for medical assessment. Heath’s findings have since been replicated in British data collected in the 1990s (Gafaranga & Britten, Citation2003); in U.S. data (Robinson, Citation2006); and, more recently, in Korean (Park, Citation2017) and Vietnamese data (Nguyen & Austin, Citation2018).

Building on these findings, several studies have considered the implications of different kinds of Type A opening question formats. For example, in a mixed methods study of physicians’ opening questions in U.S. data, Heritage and Robinson (Citation2006a) found that “general enquiry” questions (e.g., “What can I do for you today?”) were statistically associated with longer patient problem presentations and more positive post-visit evaluations of physicians’ communication (Robinson & Heritage, Citation2006) than “confirmatory” questions (e.g., “Sore throat, huh?”). Contrastingly, in the Korean data, Park (Citation2017) found physicians most frequently employed, “Eti-ka pwulphenhasyese osyesseyo?”—a format that translates as, “Where does it hurt that (made you) come in?” These “location” questions were associated with briefer problem presentations and a faster transition into history taking, possibly reflecting a “culturally specific goal-oriented progressivity” (Park, Citation2017, p. 14).

Studies of the normative organization of physicians’ opening questions have also provided a framework for understanding “problematic” communication during nonstandard consultation openings. For example, in British data from out-of-hours home visits to patients living with dementia, Dooley and Barnes (Citation2022) observed that physicians opened the consultations by stating their own reason for the visit, using information from case record entries made during the caregiver’s initial call. In Fragment (1), at Line 1 the physician informs the patient of the reason for the visit.

Fragment (1) From Dooley and Barnes (Citation2022, p. 3).

Misalignment was evident in all the home visit openings in the study, as shown in Line 4, where the patient denies the existence of current symptoms and/or their involvement in the decision to seek help.

In professional guidance, upfront agenda setting (i.e., pursuit of further concerns immediately after patients have presented their chief concern) is often recommended in a bid to increase efficiency (Brock et al., Citation2011). In a mixed methods analysis of three U.S. datasets collected between 2000 and 2005, Robinson et al. (Citation2016) examined the nature, positioning, and effectiveness of different physician questioning strategies designed to elicit additional patient concerns. They found that “concern-seeking” questions asked early in the visit were effective in generating additional agenda items, but were rarely employed (see Heritage et al., Citation2007).

The closing phase is another location where physicians might elicit further agenda items by initiating a “final-concern sequence.” However, Robinson (Citation2001a) found that the design of physicians’ final-concern questions (e.g., “Any other concerns?”) in U.S. data tended to bias answers toward “no” responses, and that the questions were often delivered during other activities (e.g., while entering information into the patient’s health record), without gazing at the patient, or even while standing up in anticipation of the patient’s exit (Robinson Citation2001a).

Patients’ agenda-setting practices

Topic initiation

Our included studies also offered distinctive insights into how patients negotiate their own agendas. For example, Heath (Citation1981) noted that patients would sometimes initiate second and occasionally third topics themselves. Campion and Langdon (Citation2004) confirmed this in British data collected in the late 1980s. They also observed that while presenting their reason for the visit, some patients used “pre-announcements” (e.g., “Well, it’s three things really”) or listing practices (e.g., “First of all …”) to give advance warning of candidate second or third topics that their physicians might either accept, postpone, or ignore. More commonly, patients would indicate second or third topics later in the visit via “opportunistic announcements,” following observable breaks in the physician’s attention or during the closing phase of the visit. Campion and Langdon (Citation2004) noted that the success of such later attempts were also subject to their acknowledgment and consideration by physicians.

Similarly, in Danish data, Beck Nielsen (Citation2012) found that patients would attempt to initiate new topics later in the visit, in response to physicians’ closing-implicative moves such as “arrangement-making sequences,” as shown in Fragment (2):

Fragment (2) From Beck Nielsen (Citation2012, p. 557).

Beck Nielsen (Citation2012) found that all additional concerns initiated by patients were addressed by physicians. Contrastingly, in Korean data, Park (Citation2013) found that physicians would push toward closure rather than engaging with patients’ attempts to topicalize additional concerns, on the rare occasion patients did so.

Patients’ ideas

Patients’ agenda-setting practices also include offering ideas about what might, or might not, be wrong, either overtly or more tacitly. This practice has been observed in Dutch, U.S., Finnish, Swedish, and British data: for example, patients suggesting a “lay” or “candidate diagnosis” (Houtkoop-Steenstra, Citation1986), articulating “diagnostic claims” or “etiological hypotheses” (Heritage & Robinson, Citation2006b), proposing a site of origin for a symptom (Gill & Maynard, Citation2006), or preemptively resisting a particular diagnosis (Gill et al., Citation2010). Fragment (3) illustrates this in Swedish data from a study of telephone calls to a nurse-led primary care line where, at Line 3, the caller tentatively suggests a candidate diagnosis in the slot where a request for help might be expected.

Fragment (3) From Leppänen (Citation2005, p. 184).

In U.S. data, Gill (Citation1998) reported that patients’ explanations were designed to downplay their knowledge, embedded within speculative questions, or else done in such a way as to avoid setting themselves up for potentially disaffiliative responses. Pomerantz et al. (Citation2007) observed a similar practice in another U.S. dataset in which patients offered medically serious conditions (e.g., a cardiac problem) as a candidate explanation for their symptoms by “displaying some modulated degree of concern or worry” (Pomerantz et al., Citation2007, p. 144), while at the same time portraying the explanation as unlikely. Exploring patients’ attempts to raise psychological explanations in Danish data, Tarber and Frostholm (Citation2015) found that their success was dependent on physician collaboration. Indeed, a key negative consequence of such a cautious approach is that “physicians may leave patients’ explanations unassessed or even unacknowledged” (Gill & Maynard, Citation2006, p. 117).

Several studies demonstrated different ways by which primary care staff can be responsive to patients’ ideas about what might be wrong by orienting to candidate diagnoses, either immediately in their next action (as shown by the nurse’s challenge at Line 6 in Fragment (3)), during the physical examination (Heritage & Stivers, Citation1999), or later on in the diagnostic (Heath, Citation1992) or treatment phase (Stivers, Citation2002). Fragment (4), taken from a study of Dutch data, illustrates this below. At the start of the visit (not shown) the patient says, “I think I may have something like a cold,” and informs the physician that he or she had tried to relieve the symptoms with a topical treatment for muscular pain. We are now post-physical examination.

Fragment (4) From ten Have (Citation1991, p. 8).

At Line 1 the physician responds to the patient’s earlier idea about what might be wrong, ruling out a respiratory infection with evidence from the physical examination, while later informing the patient at Line 30 that the causal hypothesis implied by the patient’s reported attempt at symptomatic treatment for muscular pain was plausible “after all.”

Patients’ concerns

Patients’ agendas can include emotional, social, or psychosocial concerns. Few of our included studies systematically explored patients’ expressions of illness worries or fears in terms of where in the consultation, and how, they were invoked and/or responded to. In Swedish data from nurse-led telephone triage, Leppänen (Citation2005) reported that patients sometimes overtly displayed worry in their problem presentations, taking an “emotional” as opposed to “troubles-resistant” stance. In U.S. data, Stivers and Heritage (Citation2001) observed that patients would sometimes use “narrative expansions” in their responses to physicians’ history-taking questions, allowing for “a volunteering of information that more overtly attends to the patient’s agenda of concerns” (Stivers & Heritage, Citation2001, p. 167). In Danish data, Tarber (Citation2015) found that patients would often take advantage of moments of “suspended interaction” to disclose “emotional distress induced by social circumstances, bad health or illness worries” (Tarber, Citation2015, p. 84).

Arreskov et al. (Citation2021) analyzed physician responses to patients’ emotional concerns, or questions expressing such concerns, in Danish data from annual chronic care checkups for patients with multimorbidity. They found that most physicians gave minimal affiliative or non-affiliative responses when patients attempt to introduce a concern, effectively closing them down in favor of progressing the biomedical agenda (Arreskov et al. Citation2021). The same bias toward progressing a routinized, biomedical “checklist” agenda at the expense of exploring patients’ concerns was observed in British data on nurse-led routine chronic care consultations for diabetes (Rhodes et al., Citation2006).

Patients’ expectations

In many health systems, primary care physicians and nurses play a gate-keeping role, authorizing access to other medical services. Patients therefore often come to a consultation seeking certain treatments or medical interventions they are otherwise unable to access directly (e.g., prescription-only medicines or referrals to specialty care). Several studies identified interactional practices in which a patient/caregiver conveys expectations for particular consultation outcomes. A common finding was that such practices were rarely formatted as direct requests (for instance, Gill, Citation2005). Instead, they are “frequently interactionally extended and complex, containing a myriad of component actions, such as taking, advocating, and resisting positions regarding the decision, and soliciting and providing information in the service of making the decision” (Robinson, Citation2001a, p. 20).

In U.S. data from consultations for respiratory illness, Stivers and colleagues have identified a range of more or less overt practices, including “priming,” “nudging,” and “resisting alternative treatments,” by which caregivers of child patients, or adult patients themselves, might be heard (whether intended or not) as advocating for antibiotic treatment (Stivers & Timmermans, Citation2021). In data from China on pediatric consultations for respiratory illness, overt caregiver advocating actions were more frequently observed than in the U.S. data, with inquiries about antibiotic treatment being the most common (Wang, Citation2020). Wang (Citation2020) also found that conveying a preference for a particular treatment approach could be used to push back on a physician’s recommendation. This can be seen in Fragment (5). Just prior to this, the caregiver has resisted the physician’s offer of oral antibiotics on the grounds that they have already tried this.

Fragment (5) Adapted from Wang (Citation2020, p. 10).

At Line 1 the physician informs the caregiver that the number of tablets her child has been taking was too low. Following two seconds of silence, at Line 3, the patient’s mother conveys her preference for an intravenous antibiotic infusion, thereby educing an offer from the physician at Line 4.

So far, the studies included in our review have shown that, despite being recommended in professional guidance, physicians seldom solicited additional patient problems or concerns. The studies have also shown that patients have multiple ways of voicing their own agendas, ideas, and concerns, although their success in terms of getting them addressed relies wholly on physician collaboration. This finding runs contrary to the wider PHC and professional literature, which recommends that physicians work to discover the “patient’s perspective” (Edwards et al., Citation2023). Finally, we showed how patients can employ multiple practices to convey expectations for certain consultation outcomes. In the next section we turn to our second theme: managing participation.

Managing participation

In the wider PHC literature, the concept of participation generally refers to the communicative involvement of the patient during medical visits, particularly in treatment decision making (Robinson, Citation2003). In this literature, numerous benefits of patient participation have been documented, such that promoting it is now a policy imperative across many Western countries. The implication here being that the default patient position is one of passivity. Our second research theme brings together CA research on how patients and physicians actively manage participation together using language and embodied action as resources. Here, participation is respecified as, “actions demonstrating forms of involvement performed by parties within evolving structures of talk” (Goodwin & Goodwin, Citation2005, p. 222). This theme brings together a range of interests: (1) studies informed by sociological interests in engagement in collaborative action; (2) studies informed by psychological interests in the revelation and management of affect in terms of how it is embedded in participation; and (3) studies informed by professional/practical interests, such as the impact of technologies or clinical guidelines on participation.

Engagement in collaborative action

The earliest study in this theme showed how patients can be active agents in collaborative action (Heath, Citation1982). In British data, Heath observed patients routinely employing embodied “displays of availability” at the start of medical visits, thereby establishing “co-presence” and preparedness to participate (Heath, Citation1982). Additionally, when physicians were otherwise engaged (e.g., reading the patient’s record), patients were seen to “display recipiency” (i.e., their readiness to reintroduce talk) by gazing toward the physician, thereby eliciting a response (Heath, Citation1982). Indeed, Heath found that patients employed a range of vocal and embodied actions (e.g., speech perturbation, posture shifts, and gestural activity) either juxtaposed within or alongside an ongoing utterance, to secure physician engagement (Heath, Citation1984).

Heath also demonstrated how patients used embodied actions in the service of “bodily revelations,” “showing, rather than simply describing” (Heath, Citation2018, p. 167) the problem, to invite physicians to visually orient to their difficulties. Similarly in Finnish data, Ruusuvuori (Citation2001) demonstrated how patients monitored physician engagement during the problem presentation, treating physician disengagement (e.g., to read or write in the patient record) as problematic. Patients responded with speech “dysfluencies” (e.g., pausing) and/or pointing gestures, in a bid to elicit attention and reestablish mutual involvement (Ruusuvuori Citation2001, p. 1098). As argued by Heath, “how the doctor attends to and participates in the patient’s talk may be consequential to what the patient says and consequently medical decision-making, treatment programmes and the like” (Heath, Citation1984, p. 313).

Subsequent U.S. CA studies have evidenced this argument, for example, demonstrating how history-taking sequences (Boyd & Heritage, Citation2006) can constrain patients’ talk. Yet patients are seldom passive recipients of “designedly restrictive” questions (Stivers & Heritage, Citation2001). For example, patients’ “expanded answers” during history taking have been shown to preempt possible negative inferences or criticism (Stivers & Heritage, Citation2001). An example can be seen from British data in Fragment (6), in which the patient, who has presented with “excruciating” hip pain, is asked whether she has taken anything to manage the pain before consulting.

Fragment (6) From Barnes and Van der Scheer (Citation2021, p. 39).

In Line 2, following a turn-initial “no,” the patient’s expanded answer reverses her initial response to “well yeah,” before expanding with a narrative of treatment failure, thereby proclaiming a troubles-resistant stance (Heritage & Robinson, Citation2006b) and bolstering the legitimacy of their visit.

In other activity phases, patients’ verbal participation has been found to be recurrently patterned. For example, in British data on physical examinations, Heath found that, although cooperative, patients were “seemingly disattentive” to the actions performed, turning away, adopting a “middle-distance orientation,” and withholding any response (Heath, Citation2006, p. 190). However with pain-related complaints, patients adopted a more active participatory stance to assist in revealing the pain’s location and severity (Heath, Citation2006). In U.S. data, Heritage and Stivers found that physicians’ “online commentary” practices during the physical examination were “rarely overtly addressed to patients or directly acknowledged by them” (Heritage & Stivers, Citation1999, p. 1504). Additionally, in a study of the delivery of diagnoses in British data, Heath noted “the absence of patient participation in response” (Heath, Citation1992, p. 241), unless encouraged, for example, by the physician’s expressions of uncertainty or incongruence with a candidate diagnosis or cause previously implied by patients. In contrast, U.S. data on treatment recommendations have shown they routinely require active patient verbal support and endorsement (Stivers, Citation2006).

Naturally, there is a need for all parties to negotiate disengagement at the end of the consultation. The role of patient participation in closings has been explored in British and U.S. data—for example, how physical leave taking is coordinated with patients’ acceptance of physicians’ treatment summaries (e.g., “So, the penicillin, one pill four times a day.”) or management plans (e.g., “So, when you come back in a month we’ll check your blood pressure.”) see Heath, Citation1985; Robinson, Citation2001b; West, Citation2006). These “action formulations” (i.e., treatment summaries or management plans) structurally prefer confirmation rather than encouraging any further negotiation (Gafaranga & Britten, Citation2004, Citation2007). This would seem, on the face of it, to be appropriate when closing the consultation; however, action formulations are sometimes used without any evidence of prior patient involvement in these plans earlier in the consultation (Gafaranga & Britten, Citation2004, Citation2007). Indeed, it is worth noting that “summarizing,” although broadly recommended in the professional literature (Silverman et al., Citation2013), can have unintended consequences like obstructing further patient talk (Houtkoop-Steenstra, Citation1986).

Other studies in this theme focused on how verbal participation is managed in “more-than-two-party” consultations—for example, by companions. These studies include PHC interactions with familial caregivers in U.S. pediatric consultations (e.g., Stivers, Citation2012), and with formal caregivers for patients with intellectual disabilities in British PHC consultations (e.g., Antaki & Chinn, Citation2019). For reasons of space, we do not discuss these studies here, but for recent reviews that include studies of the role of companions in PHC, see Jenkins et al. (Citation2024) and Webb and Dooley (Citation2024).

Embedding affect in participation

Several studies in this theme explored how affect is embedded in participation—that is, “the expression of emotions during the consultation, and health professionals’ responses to these displays” (Peräkylä & Ruusuvuori, Citation2007, p. 173). For example in British data, Heath (Citation2002) showed how patients often employed gestures when describing their symptoms, to express both emotional and personal experiences, such as suffering, that would otherwise remain invisible. The embedding of affect has also been shown to be accomplished more indirectly by patients when orienting to the “delicate” or “sensitive nature” of their talk. For example, Haakana (Citation2001) found in Finnish data that patients used laughter to both display and recognize a problem: “in places where they have to momentarily portray themselves in an unfavorable light” (Haakana, Citation2001, p. 213). Similarly, Tietbohl and Bergen (Citation2022) showed in U.S. data how patients employed prefaces (e.g., “The thing is … ”) to mark their upcoming talk as disclosing sensitive or embarrassing information when seeking resolution of certain problems. Although not directly displaying emotion as such, patients can be seen as actively managing affect by mitigating the impact of disclosures that might reflect negatively on themselves or the physician.

Regarding responses to displays or expressions of emotion, traditionally, Western physicians have been socialized to adopt a normative stance of “detached concern” (Lief & Fox, Citation1963), balancing objectivity and empathy. This has been evidenced in British data showing how physicians adopted an objective “analytic stance” in response to patient pain displays in order to fulfill the duties of medical assessment (Heath, Citation1989). Similarly, in Swedish data, Leppänen (Citation2010) demonstrated how nurses worked to sustain “emotional neutrality” while triaging patients, by accounting for their decisions in organizational and professional terms. However, such a stance can be problematic. To paraphrase Heritage (Citation2011), there can be “dysfunctional” consequences when interactional norms in ordinary conversation (e.g., affiliative responses to troubles talk) are absent during the medical visit. For example, in two separate U.S. studies, Stivers and Heritage (Citation2001) noted that physicians, “may not routinely affiliate with patients’ lifeworld narratives” (Stivers & Heritage, Citation2001, p. 151), and (Jones, Citation2001) observed that patients sometimes orient to such absences by leaving a gap or actively pursuing a response.

The impact of technological innovation on participation

With the rise of technological innovation inside the consulting room, PHC professionals have been increasingly expected to manage “simultaneous and often competing demands” (Heath, Citation1984, p. 312). A number of early CA studies explored the shift from paper records to electronic health record (EHR) use in terms of how it has shaped and mediated “normal rules of engagement.” For example, in British data, Robinson (Citation1998) demonstrated how embodied shifts in engagement by physicians, in order to accomplish noncollaborative tasks (i.e., the physician reading the EHR), communicated that patient-initiated actions during this time would not be treated as relevant. In data from New Zealand, Dowell et al. (Citation2013) found that transitions into longer episodes of computer use by physicians often featured contemporaneous information produced “online” (e.g., “I’ll just have a look.”). Similarly in Danish data, Beck Nielsen (Citation2014, Citation2016) noted that physicians often provided “online” explanations to establish the relevance of suspending participation to access supplementary information about patients (e.g., drug allergies), “for the potentially delicate business of validating patients’ own versions” (Beck Nielsen, Citation2016, p. 72).

In a study of physicians’ prescribing behavior in British data, Greatbatch (Citation2006) revealed a preoccupation with the task-in-hand, in which physicians synchronized the timing of prescription-related talk with related keyboard actions. Moreover, it has been found that patients simultaneously coordinate their turns-at-talk with predictable pauses in ongoing activity in such a way as to minimize possible disruption (Greatbatch, Citation2006; Greatbatch et al., Citation1995). Finally, in British data detailing the use of electronic templates in nurse-led routine consultations for chronic disease, Rhodes et al. (Citation2008) found that patient participation was inhibited when nurses did not shift engagement between patient and computer via gaze and bodily orientation.

Other studies have begun to examine the interplay between patients, physicians, and health technologies such as diagnostic instruments and paraclinical tools in PHC consultations (e.g., Ford et al., Citation2020; Lindell, Citation2017). For example, Lindell studied how point-of-care testing was introduced, used, and the results accounted for in Danish consultations for common infections (Citation2017). Although designed to assist a physician diagnosis, Lindell found the results—in particular, elevated results—were presented from the position of an unconditional, external authority (Citation2017). These studies of the “communicative use” of health technologies are useful for their critical insights (i.e., actual use vs. planned use and unintended consequences) and the evidence they have provided regarding the impact of technological innovation on participation.

The impact of professional guidance/policy interventions on participation

As well as illuminating why certain practice guidelines are challenging to implement, CA studies can also reveal unexpected consequences. A small group of studies in this theme focused on the impact of guidelines issued by professional bodies for “good clinical practice” on patient participation. In a British study of “prescribed” questions to assess suicidal ideation, Miller (Citation2013), found that physicians had to work harder to “naturalize” such questions prediagnosis, compared to when they asked them later in the consultation following a diagnosis of depression. They also found that patients had to work hard to mitigate the potentially damaging inferences arising from their responses. As well as the location of these particular questions, their design has also been shown to be interactionally consequential. For example, in another British study, Ford et al. (Citation2021) found that suicide risk assessment questions tended to be “optimized” for a “no-problem” response.

Finally, a small group of studies has considered the successful (or otherwise) diffusion into practice of PHC policy interventions aiming to promote opportunities to support patient self-management. Examples can be seen in a Dutch study of collaborative goal setting and action planning in chronic illness (Lenzen et al., Citation2018), and a Finnish study on shared decision making (Ijäs-Kallio et al., Citation2011a). In both studies, the recommended practices were either not observed at all or not implemented as intended by the policy itself. Thereby, a common finding across these studies is a gap between the “theory” and actual practice (see Pilnick, Citation2022).

In this section we have focused on the management of participation in interaction, bringing together early CA studies of collaborative action with more recent studies exploring the embedding of affect in participation, and studies evaluating the impact of technological innovations and the implementation of talk-based professional/policy interventions. A common feature here has been a multi-modal approach to the analysis of participatory actions. CA studies in this research area have made a unique contribution to the wider primary care literature by demonstrating how the concept of participation can be grounded in actual interaction and providing a robust method for the critical evaluation of prescribed talked-based interventions. In the next section, we explore our final research theme, the management of medical authority in interaction.

Managing authority

Managing authority has been a longstanding area of interest in medical sociology. The studies included here focused on documenting and understanding authority via the interactional management of “asymmetries” between patients and physicians relating to knowledge (Drew, Citation1991), initiative (Frankel, Citation1990), and task (ten Have, Citation1991). Many of the findings from these studies have clear implications for related professional/practical “problems.” In what follows, we show how CA studies that speak to asymmetries of authority have generated practical insights into four key areas: negotiating shared understanding, giving lifestyle advice, diagnosing illness, and recommending treatment.

Negotiating shared understanding

Our earliest study in this theme (Meehan, Citation1981) focused on participant orientations to the unequal distribution of, and rights or entitlement to, medical knowledge. In U.S. data, Meehan explored how the sense of medical terms and procedures could be actively negotiated between physicians and patients. As well as noting cases of self-repair by physicians to address issues of intelligibility when using medical terminology, Meehan illustrated how patients themselves would sometimes initiate repair around physician’s use of specialist vocabulary (e.g., “What is a palpitation?”). Yet, as argued by Drew, “an asymmetry of knowledge is not equivalent to ‘not knowing’” (Drew, Citation1991, p. 39); many patients regularly use and are familiar with medical terms. Despite this, Meehan (Citation1981) found patients’ own use of medical terms was often mitigated, orienting to their asymmetrical position as a non-authoritative source of that knowledge.

Examining the organization and distribution of “conditionally relevant queries” in U.S. data, West (Citation1984) found these to be evenly distributed between physicians and patients, concluding that they had “mutually accessible means for establishing intelligibility of their utterances” (West, Citation1984, p. 129). In Fragment (7)—a patient-initiated example—the physician has just checked the patient’s blood pressure.

Fragment (7) From West (Citation1984, p. 111).

The measurement announced by the physician at Line 1 is subject to a “try-marked” understanding check from the patient, who initiates repair at Line 9. At Line 13, the problem remains unresolved, and the patient locates the repairable in a different way, displaying knowledge that there is a difference between “the top number” and the bottom number.

West also noted that physicians often appended “query terms” (e.g., “Okay?” or “All right?”) to utterances that “offered explanation, gave advice, or proposed plans for future action” (West, Citation1984, p. 120). Frankel (Citation1990) observed a similar phenomenon, arguing that these physician “solicits” functioned as “last calls,” giving patients the opportunity to confirm or disconfirm intelligibility, or to ask questions before progressing to next activities. However most patient responses consisted of minimal acknowledgment tokens. Another means by which physicians have been shown to attempt to check patient understanding is with “question-seeking questions” (e.g., “Any questions?”). Despite this practice being widely recommended in professional training, Robinson et al. (Citation2016) found in U.S. data that such questions were only asked in 14% (58 out of 407) of visits, and when they were asked, they elicited an affirmative patient response on only 7% of occasions.

In U.S. data on the delivery of test results, Pomerantz and Rintel (Citation2004) demonstrated how patients negotiated unequal distribution of knowledge/expertise by requesting explanations or interpretations of technical medical information when given by physicians without interpretation (e.g., numerical formulations as in Fragment (7)), in order to seek understanding of their import (also see Montenegro & Dori-Hacohen, Citation2020). Conversely, in a study of blood sugar solicitation sequences in U.S. routine diabetes visits, Wingard et al. (Citation2014) found that physicians would request precise numbers in response to patients’ own non-numerical assessments or interpretations of blood sugar levels (e.g., when indicating no change).

Shared knowledge and understanding between patients and physicians is integral to good medical practice, and its absence may result in the potential for patient harm or litigation. The findings show how the distribution of, and rights or entitlement to, medical knowledge is negotiated in and through interaction between physicians and patients. They also demonstrate in situ approaches to monitoring patients’ understanding and their commitment to proposed courses of action.

Giving lifestyle advice

PHC professionals often need to ask patients about their diet, physical exercise, or alcohol and substance use, in order to establish whether lifestyle may be a factor in their medical problem or to provide advice about ways they can maintain and improve health or wellbeing. This topic is particularly important for patients with chronic illness, as lifestyle changes can have significant impacts on health outcomes. However, because the physician “does not control the knowledge base,” managing epistemic authority in discussions about patients’ personal choices and behaviors can sometimes be “difficult and uncomfortable” (Freeman, Citation1987, p. 962).

In a study of U.S. data, Freeman found that when physicians made an explicit connection, or personal link, between a patient’s lifestyle and their medical problem, there was less “conversational disruption” (e.g., rejection of the topic). Similar observations have been made in other U.S. data (e.g., Cohen et al., Citation2011), and in British data (e.g., Connabeer, Citation2021). In another U.S. study, Bergen (Citation2020) found that when physicians framed advice as “treatment-implicative,” it was more likely to be verbally accepted by patients than “plain advice.” An exception to this pattern was noted in British data, in a study of smoking discussions by Pilnick and Coleman (Citation2003), who found that attempts to personalize a problem that was not shared engendered patient resistance. Relatedly, in Finnish data, Sorjonen et al. (Citation2006) found that physicians were unlikely to give advice to patients regarding their lifestyle choices unless patients had highlighted a problem themselves (and their efforts to change) in their initial response to physician questions about it.

Epistemic rights are linked with personal responsibilities and moral obligations. Several studies examined how these issues were interactionally managed. One common finding was that potential moral implications regarding lifestyle choices or health behaviors can be avoided, or resisted, by patients. Sorjonen et al. (Citation2006) and Denvir (Citation2012) found in Finnish and U.S. data, respectively, that patients would often portray their substance use as “normal and healthy” during history taking, thereby implying its non-problematic character.

Fragment (8) From Sorjonen et al. (Citation2006, p. 348).

Both studies also found that patients often resisted physicians’ efforts to quantify their substance use with health-oriented descriptions or “no-problem” answers. Sorjonen et al. (Citation2006) found that the latter often resulted in extended sequences due to subsequent physician requests for specification, as shown at Line 3 in Fragment 8.

Possible moral implications were sometimes addressed head on by patients themselves. In Danish data, Guassora et al. (Citation2015) found that, in response to physicians’ questions about lifestyle risks in annual checkups for people with chronic illness, patients would sometimes give anticipatory answers, heading off potential next-turn negative inferences or evaluations by physicians, as shown in Fragment (9).

Fragment (9) From Guassora et al. (Citation2015, p. 194).

Similarly, in data from New Zealand on diabetes consultations, Barton et al. (Citation2016) found that patients either preemptively blocked likely behavior change advice or resisted it by proffering “candidate obstacles.” These obstacles provided information about the patients’ own life circumstances (e.g., family commitments or other priorities) that might act as a basis for advice resistance or even shape advice given later on in the consultation, while also doing moral work positioning the patient as “willing but unable” (also see Pilnick & Coleman, Citation2006).

In U.S. data, Dillon (Citation2012) found that patients sometimes engaged in “defensive detailing” as a means of mitigating potential moral implications of disclosed lifestyle behaviors (e.g., in relation to an unplanned pregnancy). Dillon also observed that physicians would sometimes solicit accounts from patients for “medical misdeeds,” “indexing a claim that the accountable event does not accord with common sense and is, thus, possibly inappropriate or unwarranted” (Dillon, Citation2012, p. 216). However, in U.S. data on chronic care visits, Bergen and Stivers (Citation2013) found that such patient disclosures (e.g., unhealthy behavior choices) could just as commonly be topicalized by patients themselves via “patient-initiated announcements.”

Giving lifestyle advice is widely recognized as challenging for PHC professionals. CA findings in U.S., Nordic, and British data offer clear insights into why this task might be experienced as challenging, and how asymmetries of epistemic authority and “territories of information” are recognized, asserted, and navigated by physicians and patients in situ.

Diagnosing illness

The delivery of a professional opinion in the form of a diagnosis has long been thought to represent the pinnacle of medical authority. In British data recorded in the 1970s and 1980s, Heath found that most diagnostic “informings” were delivered as an authoritative, “factual, monolithic assertion” (Heath, Citation1992, p. 246) and that, despite opportunities to do so, patients responded minimally (if at all), displaying an “extraordinary passivity” (Heath, Citation1992, p. 261). More extended patient responses featured if physicians displayed uncertainty or indexed incongruity with the patient’s ideas about what might be wrong, triggering patient’s “post-diagnostic accounts” and expanded diagnostic sequences. For example, bolstering the legitimacy of their grounds for seeking professional help in the face of a “no-problem” diagnosis (Heritage, Citation2009). Importantly however, patient’s responses “preserved the differential status between their own version and the understanding of the expert” (Heath, Citation1992, p. 249), underscoring, “the asymmetrical relationship between the participants and the differential status of their opinions concerning illness” (Heath, Citation1992, p. 264).

In Finnish data collected in the 1990s, Peräkylä (Citation1998) noted a more nuanced balance of authority and accountability. Although “plain assertions” still formed the majority of diagnostic utterances, they were usually delivered immediately adjacent to the physical examination, at which the evidential basis had observably been gathered or could be inferred (Peräkylä, Citation2006). When delivered at some distance from the physical examination, or when there was diagnostic uncertainty or potential conflict with patients’ own ideas (e.g., regarding the seriousness of their condition), physicians would incorporate more of the evidential basis within what they said (Peräkylä, Citation2006). Like Heath (Citation1992), Peräkylä (Citation2002) found that extended patient responses (whether agreeing or resisting) only featured if physicians displayed uncertainty, if there was incongruity with the patient’s ideas about what might be wrong, or when the physician explicated the evidential grounds for their diagnosis.

In U.S. data collected between 2003 and 2005, Heritage and McArthur (Citation2019) noted that diagnostic utterances were only present in around 50% of the consultations they examined. Unlike Heath (Citation1992) and Peräkylä (Citation2002), the authors found physicians delivered more “mitigated” diagnostic utterances than plain assertions. However, in line with prior research, they also found that extended patient responses were most likely when diagnoses were epistemically downgraded (Heritage & McArthur, Citation2019).

In a Finnish study, Ijäs-Kallio et al. (Citation2010) argued that patient’s resistive actions post-diagnosis (such as proffering past experience) indexed a stance that physician epistemic authority was negotiable (also see Drew, Citation2006). Similarly, in data from China, Wu (Citation2017) found that patients sometimes resisted diagnostic reasoning by drawing on their own authority to contest symptom descriptions. In Danish data, Lindell (Citation2019) found that patients sometimes introduced residual concerns post-diagnosis about symptoms already mentioned but left unexplained or unaddressed (also see Maynard & Frankel, Citation2019). Although ostensibly the placement of such residual concerns might be seen as resistive, Lindell argued they were more likely a bid for missing relevant information (i.e., concerning symptom treatability, duration, or cause) prior to closing (Maynard & Frankel, Citation2019).

By providing a detailed picture of the delivery and receipt of diagnostic utterances, CA scholars have enabled a radically different view of the expression and management of medical authority grounded in PHC interactions. Over time and across different healthcare systems, these studies have shown how physicians have moved toward balancing epistemic authority with accountability and uncertainty—moreover, that patients “can, and in a number of cases do, assume a degree of agency and knowledgeability to their diagnosis” (Peräkylä, Citation2006, p. 246).

Recommending treatment

Heritage and McArthur (Citation2019) argued that physicians and patients appear to be placing more value/attention on treatment—a solution to the problem presented—rather than diagnosis. As well as embodying epistemic authority, recommending treatment invokes “deontic” authority, the right “to effect changes in the activities of others” (Ervin-Tripp et al., Citation1984, p. 116). A number of studies have focused on how authority is encoded in treatment directives. In a U.S. study, West first highlighted that treatment directives could be formulated in different ways, ranging from “aggravated” (e.g., orders) to “mitigated” (e.g., suggestions); see West, Citation1990, p. 86). In Dutch data, ten Have (Citation1995) noted that treatment directives could be formatted via different action types with more or less authority (“proposals,” “announcements,” or “deliberate alternatives”), noting that the choice between the different formats was a likely consequence of the prior sequential environment.

In a comparative study of treatment recommendation-response sequences in U.S. and British PHC data, Stivers et al. (Citation2018) identified five different directive action types (“pronouncements,” “proposals,” “suggestions,” “offers,” and “assertions”). Stivers and colleagues showed how, “with each action type, physicians highlight the recommendation as differentially situated in epistemic and/or deontic space” (Stivers et al., Citation2018, p. 7), arguing that their selection was shaped by the specific contingencies of the consultation. According to Heritage (Citation2021), the more authoritative treatment recommendations tended to be reserved for medications that were desired by patients, or unproblematic in their administration and side effects, whereas recommendations more oriented to shared decision making were more frequently used for more problematic treatment regimens.

Unlike diagnoses, treatment recommendations are normatively built, and responded to, as a specific next action step to be accepted or rejected, for it is the patient who will ultimately decide whether or not to implement the treatment plan (Stivers, Citation2005). Several studies have demonstrated how treatment is oriented to as a negotiable matter by both patients and physicians (Stivers, Citation2006; ten Have, Citation1995). As ten Have described, during these negotiations “the GP does ‘work’ to put the patient’s acceptance firmly ‘on record,’ as an interactionally-established fact” (ten Have, Citation1995, p. 324). Yet even in the face of “doctors’ orders,” patients can and do enact agency, “by choosing how and when they endorse the recommendation” (Koenig, Citation2011, p. 1106). In a study comparing U.S. and British data, Bergen et al. (Citation2018) found contrasting cultural stances toward prescribing by patients and physicians: Whereas American patients were more likely to negotiate for prescription treatment, British patients were more likely to resist, displaying a preference for more cautious prescribing.

In many Western countries, giving patients opportunities to express their views and to play a role in treatment decision making is enshrined in professional guidance. However, in a study of Finnish primary care, Ijäs-Kallio et al. (Citation2011b) noted the “unilateral” character of treatment recommendation sequences, such that patients’ perspectives were seldom elicited. Yet they did find that patients sometimes used expanded responses to voice their own perspectives either in agreement or in negotiating the decision post-recommendation (Ijäs-Kallio et al., Citation2011b). Koenig has suggested that patient non-acceptance, “represents an opportunity for physicians to explore patient preferences and concerns” (Koenig, Citation2011, p. 112) post-recommendation. Alternatively, Barnes (Citation2018) has shown how British physicians sometimes do advance work pre-recommendation to explore patient treatment preferences and concerns. Patients’ responses to such preliminary questions often revealed their perspectives on treatment, and physicians were able to subsequently adjust their recommendations, thereby maximizing the chance of acceptance prima facie (Barnes, Citation2018).

Studies in this theme have explored the expression and management of medical authority in PHC consultations. They have provided insights into how physicians and patients collaborate in negotiating medical knowledge and understanding, the challenges inherent in asking about patients’ personal choices and behaviors, that physicians orient to accountability and index uncertainties when delivering diagnoses, and that patients can enact agency in their responses to diagnoses and recommendations for treatment. In what follows, we consider some current and future applications, issues for reflection, and the future of CA research on communication in PHC.

Applications

The fact that the themes described here all resonate with topics in wider PHC research and professional literature suggests opportunities for clinical, educational, and research applications. CA research has already been at the heart of two PHC interventions in U.S.: a cross-sectional study testing the effectiveness of two experimental questions for upfront agenda-setting in reducing patients’ unmet concerns (Heritage et al., Citation2007) and a randomized controlled trial testing the effectiveness of a communication-based distance-learning program in reducing inappropriate antibiotic prescribing for respiratory illness (Kronman et al., Citation2020). There is clearly scope as well for making evidence-based recommendations from CA studies of PHC via “informal” interventions (Robinson & Heritage, Citation2014, p. 202)—for example, the British “Antimicrobial stewardship out-of-hours” communication training program for primary care professionals (see the OPEN Project, Citation2021).

Some of the findings from studies reported here (e.g., the design of agenda-setting questions) have already enjoyed exposure in educational settings, featuring in key texts for medical communication skills training (see Silverman et al., Citation2013). Other findings could usefully inform future clinical communication curricula for both undergraduate medical training and PHC specialty training—for example, specific areas of communication known to be challenging (e.g., discussing sensitive issues or responding to emotion displays), particular clinical topics (e.g., asking about smoking, weight, and alcohol or substance use), giving behavior change advice, and skills in “conducting an effective consultation whilst managing the ‘third party’ presence of the computer” (Noble et al., Citation2018, p. 1716). The application of CA methods could also play a key role in future implementation research, helping to explain the successes, failures, and unexpected consequences of talk-based policy interventions and providing grounds for new dialogs with policy makers.

Issues for reflection

PHC entails a continuum from health promotion and disease prevention to treatment, rehabilitation, and palliative care. For practical reasons, we restricted our review to empirical studies of the provision of treatment for minor illnesses and long-term conditions. We also limited our search to studies published in English. Our findings, therefore, should not be seen as representing the field in its entirety. Also, given the number of studies meeting the inclusion criteria, our synthesis was necessarily broad, and some studies were more difficult to place than others. Due to the exploratory nature of our review, it was not appropriate to assess the quality of our included studies. Nevertheless, in collecting and examining the state of knowledge to-date, a number of the field’s strengths and weaknesses have become clear.

In terms of strengths, our review is a testament to the contributions made by the field to mapping the unique interactional “fingerprint” of communication in PHC (Heritage, Citation2004, p. 225), and providing original insights into topics of longstanding interest to primary care researchers. For example, building on the work of pioneers in the study of ordinary conversation such as Charles Goodwin, CA scholars have provided highly distinctive and compelling characterizations of the embodied collaborative management of patient participation. CA studies of PHC have also helped to expand our methodological reach as a discipline, by demonstrating the power of mixed-methods CA formal coding studies of interaction (e.g., Stivers & Barnes, Citation2018). Finally, several studies have provided evidence of gaps between professional theories, policies, and “good practice” guidance, and routine practices and actions carried out in situ. As argued by Pilnick (Citation2022), these failures can be tracked back to a lack of understanding of how talk works.

Regarding weaknesses in the field, despite the studies originating from 16 different countries/health systems, with data in 17 languages, low- and middle-income countries were hugely under-represented, and four of the languages were English variants. Most data collection was opportunistic and, although provider and patient characteristics may have been collected originally, they were often not adequately reported. When provided, patient demographics usually included age and gender, but less often ethnicity and socioeconomic status. Older patients, children, young people, and persons lacking capacity to consent were under-represented. More precise reporting and embedding inclusion in our study designs would increase the quality and relevance of our work. We also currently rely overwhelmingly on cross-sectional research designs to collect the data on which our observations are based. In order to optimize our contributions, “these observations should ideally be considered in contexts that are both comparative and historical” (Heritage, Citation2019, p. 111).

The future of CA research in primary care

Recent years have seen a dramatic increase in remote consulting (Murphy et al., Citation2021) and changes in the organization and delivery of PHC, due to trends toward task shifting and task sharing with nonmedical professionals (e.g., physician assistants/associates, paramedics, and pharmacists). Building on our cumulative scientific knowledge, future CA research should investigate the impact of these changes. Areas of interest might include the management of participation in telephone consulting (see Seuren et al., Citation2024) or the management of authority in consultations with nonmedical professionals—for example, how diagnoses and treatment recommendations are delivered, and how they are responded to by patients who may perceive those team members as holding less authority than physicians.

Although our review themes resonate with existing PHC research interests, topics such as the management of patient risk and health inequity were notably absent. Both topics are linked to the quality and experience of care patients receive during contacts with PHC professionals. Future research in these areas should consider employing mixed methods, so that demographic data and post-consultation information about what happened next can be understood in relation to interactional practices and actions. Such an approach would help to identify groups more vulnerable to harm, as well as interactional causes and manifestations of health inequities (see Stivers & Majid, Citation2007).

Comparative research in different health systems and different cultural contexts can reveal novel insights into global health issues like factors influencing antibiotic prescribing. A collaborative, programmatic approach—as opposed to isolated efforts in individual high-income countries—would enable focused research in distinct workstreams to systematically address different components of an overarching project theme. A good example of this is the, “Touch and affect in health care interaction”Footnote3 project, which is analyzing the role of touch and emotion in different types of primary care in three different cultural contexts (Finland, China, and U.K.). We should also consider combining existing datasets or collecting new longitudinal datasets as a window into historical change in primary care, examining how specific interactional practices vary over time and under different sociocultural conditions (Clayman & Heritage, Citation2021).

Supplemental material

Supplemental Material

Download PDF (294.2 KB)

Acknowledgement

The authors wish to acknowledge Nia Roberts, Information Specialist at the University of Oxford who assisted with the development of our search strategy. We also thank Ruth Parry, Anna Lindström and two anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments.

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.

Supplementary material

Supplemental data for this article can be accessed online at https://doi.org/10.1080/08351813.2024.2305038.

Correction Statement

This article has been corrected with minor changes. These changes do not impact the academic content of the article.

Additional information

Funding

Rebecca Barnes is funded by a National Institute for Health and Care Research (NIHR) Advanced Fellowship. The views expressed in this publication are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NIHR, NHS or the UK Department of Health and Social [NIHR302557].

Notes

References

  • Antaki, C., & Chinn, D. (2019). Companions’ dilemma of intervention when they mediate between patients with intellectual disabilities and health staff. Patient Education and Counseling, 102(11), 2024–2030. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2019.05.020
  • Arreskov, A. B., Lindell, J. F., & Davidsen, A. S. (2021). General practitioner responses to concerns in chronic care consultations for patients with a history of cancer. Journal of Health Psychology, 27(10), 2261–2275. https://doi.org/10.1177/13591053211025593
  • Barnes, R. K. (2018). Preliminaries to treatment recommendations in UK primary care: A vehicle for shared decision making? Health Communication, 33(11), 1366–1376. https://doi.org/10.1080/10410236.2017.1350915
  • Barnes, R. K., & Van der Scheer, I. Z. (2021). Conversation analysis: Questioning patients about prior self-treatment. In B. Gavin & H. Daniel (Eds.), Analysing health communication: Discourse approaches (pp. 19–48). Palgrave Macmillan UK.
  • Barry, C. A., Bradley, C. P., Britten, N., Stevenson, F. A., & Barber, N. (2000). Patients’ unvoiced agendas in general practice consultations: Qualitative study. BMJ, 320(7244), 1246–1250. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.320.7244.1246
  • Barton, J., Dew, K., Dowell, A., Sheridan, N., Kenealy, T., Macdonald, L., Docherty, B., Tester, R., Raphael, D., Gray, L., & Stubbe, M. (2016). Patient resistance as a resource: Candidate obstacles in diabetes consultations. Sociology of Health & Illness, 38(7), Article 7. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9566.12447
  • Beck Nielsen, S. (2012). Patient initiated presentations of additional concerns. Discourse Studies, 14(5), 549–565. https://doi.org/10.1177/1461445612454081
  • Beck Nielsen, S. (2016). How doctors manage consulting computer records while interacting with patients. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 49(1), 58–74. https://doi.org/10.1080/08351813.2016.1126451
  • Beck Nielsen, S. (2014). I’ll just see what you had before. In M. Neville, P. Haddington, T. Heinemann, & M. Rauniomaa (Eds.), Interacting with objects: Language, materiality, and social activity (pp. 79–98). John Benjamins Publishing Company.
  • Bergen, C. (2020). The conditional legitimacy of behavior change advice in primary care. Social Science and Medicine, 255, 112985. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2020.112985
  • Bergen, C., & Stivers, T. (2013). Patient disclosure of medical misdeeds. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 54(2), 221–240. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022146513487379
  • Bergen, C., Stivers, T., Barnes, R. K., Heritage, J., McCabe, R., Thompson, L., & Toerien, M. (2018). Closing the deal: A cross-cultural comparison of treatment resistance. Health Communication, 33(11), 1377–1388. https://doi.org/10.1080/10410236.2017.1350917
  • Boyd, E., & Heritage, J. (2006). Taking the history: Questioning during comprehensive history taking. In J. Heritage & D. W. Maynard (Eds.), Communication in medical care: Interaction between primary care physicians and patients (pp. 151–184). Cambridge University Press.
  • Brock, D. M., Mauksch, L. B., Witteborn, S., Hummel, J., Nagasawa, P., & Robins, L. S. (2011). Effectiveness of intensive physician training in upfront agenda setting. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 26(11), 1317–1323. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11606-011-1773-y
  • Campion, P., & Langdon, M. (2004). Achieving multiple topic shifts in primary care medical consultations: A conversation analysis study in UK general practice. Sociology of Health and Illness, 26(1), 81–101. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9566.2004.00379.x
  • Clayman, S. E., & Heritage, J. (2021). Conversation analysis and the study of sociohistorical change. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 54(2), 225–240. https://doi.org/10.1080/08351813.2021.1899717
  • Connabeer, K. (2021). Lifestyle advice in UK primary care consultations: Doctors’ use of conditional forms of advice. Patient Education and Counseling, 104(11), 2706–2715. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2021.03.033
  • Cohen, D. J., Clark, E. C., Lawson, P. J., Casucci, B. A., & Flocke, S. A. (2011). Identifying teachable moments for health behavior counseling in primary care. Patient Education and Counseling, 85(2), e8–e15. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2010.11.009
  • Denvir, P. M. (2012). When patients portray their conduct as normal and healthy: An interactional challenge for thorough substance use history taking. Social Science & Medicine, 75(9), Article 9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2012.06.021
  • Dillon, P. J. (2012). Moral accounts and membership categorization in primary care medical interviews. Communication and Medicine, 8(3), 211–222. https://doi.org/10.1558/cam.v8i3.211
  • Dooley, J., & Barnes, D. R. (2022). Negotiating ‘the problem’ in GP home visits to people with dementia. Social Science and Medicine, 298, 114862. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2022.114862
  • Dooley, J. & Webb, J. (2024). ‘Atypical interactions’ in healthcare: A state-of-the-art review of conversation-analytic research, with reflections on equity, diversity and inclusion. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 57.
  • Dowell, A., Stubbe, M., Scott-Dowell, K., Macdonald, L., & Dew, K. (2013). Talking with the alien: Interaction with computers in the GP consultation. Australian Journal of Primary Health, 19(4), 275–282. https://doi.org/10.1071/PY13036
  • Drew, P. (1991). Asymmetries of knowledge in conversational interactions. In I. Markova & K. Foppa (Eds.), Asymmetries in dialogue (pp. 21–49). Harvester Wheatsheaf.
  • Drew, P. (2006). Misalignments in “after-hours” calls to a British GP’s practice: A study in telephone medicine. In D. W. Maynard & J. Heritage (Eds.), Communication in medical care: Interaction between primary care physicians and patients (pp. 416–444). Cambridge University Press; Cambridge Core.
  • Edwards, P. J., Sellers, G. M., Leach, I., Holt, L., Ridd, M. J., Payne, R. A., & Barnes, R. K. (2023). Ideas, concerns, expectations, and effects on life (ICEE) in GP consultations: An observational study using video-recorded UK consultations. BJGP Open, 7(4), BJGPO.2023.0008. https://doi.org/10.3399/BJGPO.2023.0008
  • Ekberg, K., Beach, W., & Jones, D. (2024). Communication in outpatient secondary care: A state-of-the-art review of conversation-analytic research. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 57.
  • Ervin-Tripp, S. M., O’Connor, M. C., Rosenberg, J., Kramerae, C., & Schulz, M. (1984). Language and power in the family. In C. Kramerae & M. Schulz (Eds.), Language and power (pp. 116–135). Sage Press.
  • Ford, J., Thomas, F., Byng, R., & McCabe, R. (2020). Use of the Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ-9) in practice: Interactions between patients and physicians. Qualitative Health Research, 30(13), 2146–2159. https://doi.org/10.1177/1049732320924625
  • Ford, J., Thomas, F., Byng, R., & McCabe, R. (2021). Asking about self-harm and suicide in primary care: Moral and practical dimensions. Patient Education and Counseling, 104(4), 826–835. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2020.09.037
  • Frankel, R. M. (1990). Talking in interviews: A dispreference for patient-initiated questions in physician-patient encounters. In G. Psathas (Ed.), Interactional competence (pp. 231–262). University Press of America.
  • Freeman, S. H. (1987). Health promotion talk in family practice encounters. Social Science & Medicine, 25(8), 961–966. https://doi.org/10.1016/0277-9536(87)90267-X
  • Gafaranga, J., & Britten, N. (2003). “Fire away”: The opening sequence in general practice consultations. Family Practice, 20(3), Article 3. https://doi.org/10.1093/fampra/cmg303
  • Gafaranga, J., & Britten, N. (2004). Formulation in general practice consultations. Text & Talk, 24(2), 147–170.
  • Gafaranga, J., & Britten, N. (2007). Patient participation in formulating and opening sequences. In S. Collins, N. Britten, J. Ruusuvuori, & A. Thompson (Eds.), Patient participation in health care consultations: Qualitative perspectives (pp. 104–120). Open University Press.
  • Gill, V. T. (1998). Doing attributions in medical interaction: Patients’ explanations for illness and doctors’ responses. Social Psychology Quarterly, 61(4), 342. https://doi.org/10.2307/2787034
  • Gill, V. T. (2005). Patient ‘demand’ for medical interventions: Exerting pressure for an offer in a primary care clinic visit. Research on Language & Social Interaction, 38(4), 451–479. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327973rlsi3804_3
  • Gill, V.T., & Maynard, D. W. (2006). Explaining illness: Patients’ proposals and physicians’ responses. In D. W. Maynard & J. Heritage (Eds.), Communication in medical care: Interaction between primary care physicians and patients (pp. 115–150). Cambridge University Press; Cambridge Core.
  • Gill, V.T., Pomerantz, A., & Denvir, P. (2010). Pre-emptive resistance: Patients’ participation in diagnostic sense-making activities. Sociology of Health & Illness, 32(1), 1–20. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9566.2009.01208.x
  • Goodwin, C., & Goodwin, M. H. (2005). Participation. In D. AlessandroA Companion to Linguistic Anthropology (pp. 222–244). Blackwell Publishing Ltd.
  • Greatbatch, D., Heath, C., Campion, P., & Luff, P. (1995). How do desk-top computers affect the doctor-patient interaction. Family Practice, 12(1), 32–36. https://doi.org/10.1093/fampra/12.1.32
  • Greatbatch, D. (2006). Prescriptions and prescribing: Coordinating talk- and text-based activities. In J. Heritage & D. W. Maynard (Eds.), Communication in medical care: Interaction between primary care physicians and patients (pp. 313–339). Cambridge University Press; Cambridge Core.
  • Guassora, A. D., Nielsen, S. B., & Reventlow, S. (2015). Deciding if lifestyle is a problem: GP risk assessments or patient evaluations? A conversation analytic study of preventive consultations in general practice. Scandinavian Journal of Primary Health Care, 33(3), 191–198. https://doi.org/10.3109/02813432.2015.1078564
  • Haakana, M. (2001). Laughter as a patient’s resource: Dealing with delicate aspects of medical interaction. Text, 21(1), 187–219.
  • Heath, C. (1981). The opening sequence in doctor-patient interaction. In P. Atkinson & C. Heath (Eds.), Medical work: Realities and routines (pp. 71–90). Gower.
  • Heath, C. (1982). The display of recipiency: An instance of sequential relationship in speech and body movement. Semiotica, 42(2–4), 147–161. https://doi.org/10.1515/semi.1982.42.2-4.147
  • Heath, C. (1984). Participation in the medical consultation: The co-ordination of verbal and nonverbal behavior between the doctor and patient. Sociology of Health & Illness, 6(3), 311–338. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9566.ep10491964
  • Heath, C. (1985). The consultation’s end: The coordination of speech and body movement. International Journal of the Sociology of Language, 51, 27–42. https://doi.org/10.1515/ijsl.1985.51.27
  • Heath, C. (1989). Pain talk: The expression of suffering in the medical consultation. Social Psychology Quarterly, 52(2), 113. https://doi.org/10.2307/2786911
  • Heath, C. (2002). Demonstrative suffering: The gestural (re)embodiment of symptoms. Journal of Communication, 52(3), 597–616. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2002.tb02564.x
  • Heath, C. (2018). Embodying action: Gaze, mutual gaze and the revelation of signs and symptoms during the medical consultation. In F. Donald (Ed.), Co-operative engagements in intertwined semiosis: Essays in honour of charles goodwin (pp. 164–177). University of Tartu Press.
  • Heath, C. (1992). The delivery and reception of diagnosis in the general practice consultation. In P. Drew & J. Heritage (Eds.), Talk at work: Interaction in institutional settings (pp. 235–267). Cambridge University Press.
  • Heath, C. (2006). Body work: The collaborative production of the clinical object. In D. W. Maynard & J. Heritage (Eds.), Communication in medical care: Interaction between primary care physicians and patients (pp. 185–213). Cambridge University Press; Cambridge Core.
  • Heritage, J. (2011). The interaction order and clinical practice: Some observations on dysfunctions and action steps. Patient Education & Counseling, 84(3), 338–343.
  • Heritage, J.C. (2009). Negotiating the legitimacy of medical problems: A multi-phase concern for patients and physicians. In D. Brashers & D. Goldsmith (Eds.), Communicating to manage health and illness (pp. 147–164). Routledge.
  • Heritage, J. (2019). The expression of authority in US primary care: Offering diagnoses and recommending treatment. In C. Gordon (Ed.), Proceedings of the Georgetown round table on languages and linguistics (pp. 104–122). Georgetown University Press.
  • Heritage, J. (2021). The expression of authority in US primary care: Offering diagnoses and recommending treatment. In C. Gordon (Ed.), Approaches to discourse analysis (pp. 104–122). Georgetown University Press.
  • Heritage, J., & McArthur, A. (2019). The diagnostic moment: A study in US primary care. Social Science & Medicine, 228, 262–271. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2019.03.022
  • Heritage, J. C., & Robinson, J. D. (2006a). The structure of patients’ presenting concerns: Physicians’ opening questions. Health Communication, 19(2), 89–102. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327027hc1902_1
  • Heritage, J., Robinson, J. D., Elliott, M. N., Beckett, M., & Wilkes, M. (2007). Reducing patients’ unmet concerns in primary care: The difference one word can make. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 22(10), 1429–1433. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11606-007-0279-0
  • Heritage, J.C., & Robinson, J. D. (2006b). Accounting for the visit: Giving reasons for seeking medical care. In J. C. Heritage & D. W. Maynard (Eds.), Communication in medical care: Interactions between primary care physicians and patients (pp. 48–85). Cambridge University Press.
  • Heritage, J. (2004). Conversation analysis and institutional talk: Analyzing data. In D. Silverman (Ed.), Qualitative research: Issues of theory, method and practice (2nd ed., pp. 222–245). Sage Publications.
  • Heritage, J., & Stivers, T. (1999). Online commentary in acute medical visits: A method of shaping patient expectations. Social Science & Medicine, 49(11), 1501–1517. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0277-9536(99)00219-1
  • Houtkoop-Steenstra, H. (1986). Summarizing in doctor-patient interaction. In E. Ensink, A. van Essen, & T. van der Geest (Eds.), Discourse analysis and public life: The political interview and doctor-patient conversation. Papers from the Groningen Conference on Medical and Political Discourse (pp. 201–224). De Gruyter Mouton.
  • Ijäs-Kallio, T., Ruusuvuori, J., & Peräkylä, A. (2010). Patient resistance towards diagnosis in primary care: Implications for concordance. Health, 14(5), 505–522. https://doi.org/10.1177/1363459309360798
  • Ijäs-Kallio, T., Ruusuvuori, J., & Peräkylä, A. (2011a). Patient involvement in problem presentation and diagnosis delivery in primary care. Communication & Medicine, 7(2), 131–141. https://doi.org/10.1558/cam.v7i2.131
  • Ijäs-Kallio, T., Ruusuvuori, J., & Peräkylä, A. (2011b). ‘Unilateral’ decision making and patient participation in primary care. Communication and Medicine, 8(2), 145–155. https://doi.org/10.1558/cam.v8i2.145
  • Jenkins, L., Ekberg, S., & Wang, N. (2024). Communication in paediatric healthcare: A state-of-the-art literature review of conversation-analytic research. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 57(1), 91–108.
  • Jones, C. M. (2001). Missing assessments: Lay and professional orientations in medical interviews. Text & Talk, 21(1–2), 113–150.
  • Koenig, C. J. (2011). Patient resistance as agency in treatment decisions. Social Science & Medicine, 72(7), 1105–1114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2011.02.010
  • Kronman, M. P., Gerber, J. S., Grundmeier, R. W., Zhou, C., Robinson, J. D., Heritage, J., Stout, J., Burges, D., Hedrick, B., Warren, L., Shalowitz, M., Shone, L. P., Steffes, J., Wright, M., Fiks, A. G., & Mangione-Smith, R. (2020). Reducing antibiotic prescribing in primary care for respiratory illness. Pediatrics, 146(3), e20200038. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2020-0038
  • Lenzen, S. A., Stommel, W., Daniëls, R., van Bokhoven, M. A., van der Weijden, T., & Beurskens, A. (2018). Ascribing patients a passive role: Conversation analysis of practice nurses’ and patients’ goal setting and action planning talk. Research in Nursing and Health, 41(4), 389–397. https://doi.org/10.1002/nur.21883
  • Leppänen, V. (2005). Callers’ presentations of problems in telephone calls to Swedish primary care. In C. D. Baker, M. Emmison, & A. Firth (Eds.), Calling for help: Language and social interaction in telephone helplines (pp. 177–205). John Benjamins Publishing Company.
  • Leppänen, V. (2010). Emotional neutrality as an interactional achievement: A conversation analysis of primary care telenursing. In S. Barbara & W. Åsa (Eds.), Emotionalizing Organizations and Organizing Emotions (pp. 251–271). Palgrave Macmillan.
  • Levenstein, J. H., McCracken, E. C., McWhinney, I. R., Stewart, M. A., & Brown, J. B. (1986). The patient-centred clinical method. 1. A model for the doctor-patient interaction in family medicine. Family Practice, 3(1), 24–30. https://doi.org/10.1093/fampra/3.1.24
  • Leydon, G. M., Stuart, B., Summers, R. H., Little, P., Ekberg, S., Stevenson, F., Chew-Graham, C. A., Brindle, L., Heritage, J., Drew, P., & Moore, M. V. (2018). Findings from a feasibility study to improve GP elicitation of patient concerns in UK general practice consultations. Patient Education and Counseling, 101(8), 1394–1402. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2018.03.009
  • Lief, H. I., & Fox, R. (1963). Training for ‘detached concern’ in medical students. In H. I. Lief, V. Lief, & N. Lief (Eds.), The psychological basis of medical practice (pp. 12–35). Hoeber Medical Division of Harper & Row.
  • Lindell, J. (2017). Testing for resistance: Point-of-care testing as a communicational tool in antibiotic prescribing. Communication and Medicine, 14(3), 229–240. https://doi.org/10.1558/cam.32191
  • Lindell, J. (2019). Talk on cough: Symptom, sign and significance in acute primary care. In C. S. Jensen, S. B. Nielsen, & L. Fynbo (Eds.), Risking antimicrobial resistance: A collection of one-health studies of antibiotics and its social and health consequences (pp. 61–77). Springer International Publishing.
  • Manning, P., & Ray, G. B. (2002). Setting the agenda: An analysis of negotiation strategies in clinical talk. Health Communication, 14(4), 451–473. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327027HC1404_3
  • Maynard, D. W., Frankel, R. M. (2019). On diagnostic rationality: Bad news, good news, and the symptom residue. In D. W. Maynard & J. Heritage (Eds.), Communication in medical care: Interaction between primary care physicians and patients (pp. 248–278). Cambridge University Press; Cambridge Core.
  • Meehan, A. J. (1981). Some conversational features of the use of medical terms by doctors and patients. In P. Atkinson & C. Heath (Eds.), Medical work: Realities and routines (pp. 107–127). Gower.
  • Miller, P. K. (2013). Depression, sense and sensitivity: On pre-diagnostic questioning about self-harm and suicidal inclination in the primary care consultation. Communication and Medicine, 10(1), 37–49. https://doi.org/10.1558/cam.v10i1.37
  • Montenegro, R., & Dori-Hacohen, G. (2020). Morality in sugar talk: Presenting blood glucose levels in routine diabetes medical visits. Social Science & Medicine, 253, 112925. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2020.112925
  • Murphy, M., Scott, L. J., Salisbury, C., Turner, A., Scott, A., Denholm, R., Lewis, R., Iyer, G., Macleod, J., & Horwood, J. (2021). Implementation of remote consulting in UK primary care following the COVID-19 pandemic: A mixed-methods longitudinal study. British Journal of General Practice, 71(704), e166–e177.
  • Nguyen, H., & Austin, G. (2018). Follow-up visits in doctor-patient communication: The Vietnamese case. International Journal of Society, Culture & Language, 6(1), 18–30.
  • Noble, L. M., Scott-Smith, W., O’Neill, B., & Salisbury, H. (2018). Consensus statement on an updated core communication curriculum for UK undergraduate medical education. Patient Education and Counseling, 101(9), 1712–1719. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2018.04.013
  • OPEN Project. (2021, March). Antimicrobial stewardship out of hours. NHS England, eLearning for Health. https://www.e-lfh.org.uk/programmes/antimicrobial-stewardship-ams-out-of-hours/
  • Ouzzani, M., Hammady, H., Fedorowicz, Z. & Elmagarmid, A. (2016). Rayyan – A web and mobile app for systematic reviews. Systematic Reviews, 5(210). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13643-016-0384-4
  • Park, Y. (2013). Negotiating last-minute concerns in closing Korean medical encounters: The use of gaze, body and talk. Social Science and Medicine, 97, 176–191. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2013.08.027
  • Park, Y. (2017). A closing-implicative practice in Korean primary medical care openings. Journal of Pragmatics, 108, 1–16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2016.10.004
  • Peräkylä, A. (1998). Authority and accountability: The delivery of diagnosis in primary health care. Social Psychology Quarterly, 61(4), 301–320. https://doi.org/10.2307/2787032
  • Peräkylä, A. (2002). Agency and authority: Extended responses to diagnostic statements in primary care encounters. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 35(2), 219–247. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327973RLSI3502_5
  • Peräkylä, A. (2006). Communicating and responding to diagnosis. In D. W. Maynard & J. Heritage (Eds.), Communication in medical care: Interaction between primary care physicians and patients (pp. 214–247). Cambridge University Press; Cambridge Core.
  • Peräkylä, A., & Ruusuvuori, J. (2007). Components of participation in health care consultations: A conceptual model for research. In S. Collins, N. Britten, J. Ruusuvuori, & A. Thompson (Eds.), Patient participation in health care consultations: Qualitative perspectives (pp. 167–175). Open University Press.
  • Pilnick, A. (2022). Reconsidering patient centred care: Between autonomy and abandonment. Emerald Publishing Limited.
  • Pilnick, A., & Coleman, T. (2003). “I’ll give up smoking when you get me better”: Patients’ resistance to attempts to problematise smoking in general practice (GP) consultations. Social Science & Medicine, 57(1), 135–145. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0277-9536(02)00336-2
  • Pilnick, A., & Coleman, T. (2006). Death, depression and ‘defensive expansion’: Closing down smoking as an issue for discussion in GP consultations. Social Science & Medicine, 62(10), 2500–2512. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2005.10.031
  • Pomerantz, A., Gill, V. T., & Denvir, P. M. (2007). When patients present serious health conditions as unlikely: Managing potentially conflicting issues and constraints. In A. Hepburn & S. Wiggins (Eds.), Discursive research in practice: New approaches to psychology and interaction (pp. 127–146). Cambridge University Press.
  • Pomerantz, A., & Rintel, E. S. (2004). Practices for reporting and responding to test results during medicalconsultations: Enacting the roles of paternalism and independent expertise. Discourse Studies, 6(1), Article 1. https://doi.org/10.1177/1461445604039437
  • Rhodes, P., Langdon, M., Rowley, E., Wright, J., & Small, N. (2006). What does the use of a computerized checklist mean for patient-centered care? The example of a routine diabetes review. Qualitative Health Research, 16(3), 353–376. https://doi.org/10.1177/1049732305282396
  • Rhodes, P., Small, N., Rowley, E., Langdon, M., Ariss, S., & Wright, J. (2008). Electronic medical records in diabetes consultations: Participants’ gaze as an interactional resource. Qualitative Health Research, 18(9), 1247–1263. https://doi.org/10.1177/1049732308321743
  • Riou, M. (2024). Communication in prehospital and emergency care: A state-of-the-art literature review of conversation-analytic research, Research on Language and Social Interaction, 57(1), 55–72.
  • Robinson, J. D. (1998). Getting down to business: Talk, gaze, and body orientation during openings of doctor-patient consultations. Human Communication Research, 25(1), 97–123. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2958.1998.tb00438.x
  • Robinson, J. D. (2001a). Asymmetry in action: Sequential resources in the negotiation of a prescription request. Text & Talk, 21(1–2), 19–54.
  • Robinson, J. D. (2001b). Closing medical encounters: Two physician practices and their implications for the expression of patients’ unstated concerns. Social Science and Medicine, 53(5), 639–656. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0277-9536(00)00366-X
  • Robinson, J.D. (2003). An interactional structure of medical activities during acute visits and its implications for patients’ participation. Health Communication, 15(1), 27–59. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327027HC1501_2
  • Robinson, J. D., & Heritage, J. (2006). Physicians’ opening questions and patients’ satisfaction. Patient Education and Counseling, 60(3), 279–285. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2005.11.009
  • Robinson, J. D., & Heritage, J. C. (2014). Intervening with conversation analysis: The case of medicine. Research on Language & Social Interaction, 47(3), 201–218. https://doi.org/10.1080/08351813.2014.925658
  • Robinson, J. D. (2006). Soliciting patients’ presenting concerns. In J. C. Heritage & D. W. Maynard (Eds.), Communication in medical care: Interactions between primary care physicians and patients (pp. 22–47). Cambridge University Press.
  • Robinson, J. D., Tate, A., & Heritage, J. (2016). Agenda-setting revisited: When and how do primary-care physicians solicit patients’ additional concerns? Patient Education and Counseling, 99(5), 718–723. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2015.12.009
  • Ruusuvuori, J. (2001). Looking means listening: Coordinating displays of engagement in doctor-patient interaction. Social Science & Medicine, 52(7), 1093–1108. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0277-9536(00)00227-6
  • Seuren, L., Ilomäki, S., Dalmaijer, E., Shaw, S., & Stommel, W. (2024). Communication in telehealth: A state-of-the-art literature review of conversation-analytic research. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 57(1), 73–90.
  • Silverman, J., Kurtz, S., & Draper, J. (2013). Skills For Communicating With Patients (3rd ed.). Routledge.
  • Sorjonen, M.-L., Raevaara, L., Haakana, M., Tammi, T., & Peräkylä, A. (2006). Lifestyle discussions in medical interviews. In D. W. Maynard & J. Heritage (Eds.), Communication in medical care: Interaction between primary care physicians and patients (pp. 340–378). Cambridge University Press; Cambridge Core.
  • Stivers, T. (2002). Presenting the problem in pediatric encounters: ‘Symptoms only’ versus ‘candidate diagnosis’ presentations. Health Communication, 14(3), Article 3. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327027HC1403_2
  • Stivers, T. (2005). Parent resistance to physicians’ treatment recommendations: One resource for initiating a negotiation of the treatment decision. Health Communication, 18(1), 41–74. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327027hc1801_3
  • Stivers, T. (2006). Treatment decisions: Negotiations between doctors and patients in acute care encounters. In J. C. Heritage & D. W. Maynard (Eds.), Communication in medical care: Interactions between primary care physicians and patients (pp. 248–278). Cambridge University Press.
  • Stivers, T. (2012). Physician-child interaction: When children answer physicians’ questions in routine medical encounters. Patient Education and Counseling, 87(1), 3–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2011.07.007
  • Stivers, T., & Barnes, R. K. (2018). Treatment recommendation actions, contingencies, and responses: An introduction. Health Communication, 33(11), 1331–1334. https://doi.org/10.1080/10410236.2017.1350914
  • Stivers, T., & Heritage, J. (2001). Breaking the sequential mold: Answering ‘more than the question’ during comprehensive history taking. Text – Interdisciplinary Journal for the Study of Discourse, 21(1–2), Article 1–2. https://doi.org/10.1515/text.1.21.1-2.151
  • Stivers, T., Heritage, J., Barnes, R. K., McCabe, R., Thompson, L., & Toerien, M. (2018). Treatment recommendations as actions. Health Communication, 33(11), 1335–1344.
  • Stivers, T., & Majid, A. (2007). Questioning children: Interactional evidence of implicit bias in medical interviews. Social Psychology Quarterly, 70(4), Article 4. https://doi.org/10.1177/019027250707000410
  • Stivers, T., & Timmermans, S. (2021). Arriving at no: Patient pressure to prescribe antibiotics and physicians’ responses. Social Science & Medicine, 290, 114007. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2021.114007
  • Tarber, C. (2015). Seize the moment! Moments of suspended interaction as a patient resource for introducing psychosocial problems. Academic Quarter | Akademisk Kvarter, 12, 83–101. https://journals.aau.dk/index.php/ak/article/view/2729/2187
  • Tarber, C., & Frostholm, L. (2015). Disclosure of mental health problems in general practice: The gradual emergence of latent topics and resources for achieving their consideration. Communication and Medicine, 11(2), 189–202. https://doi.org/10.1558/cam.v11i2.17404
  • ten Have, P. (1991). Talk and institution: A reconsideration of the ‘asymmetry’ of doctor-patient interaction. In D. Boden & D. H. Zimmerman (Eds.), Talk and social structure: Studies in ethnomethodology and conversation analysis (pp. 138–163). Polity Press.
  • ten Have, P. (1995). Disposal negotiations in general practice consultations. In A. Firth (Ed.), The discourse of negotiation: Studies of language in the workplace (pp. 319–344). Pergamon.
  • Tietbohl, C., & Bergen, C. (2022). Talking about things: A patient cue for sensitive healthcare problems and effective physician responses. Health Communication, 38(9), 1–8. https://doi.org/10.1080/10410236.2022.2046920
  • Wang, N. C. (2020). Understanding antibiotic overprescribing in China: A conversation analysis approach. Social Science & Medicine, 262, 113251. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2020.113251
  • West, C. (1984). Medical misfires: Mishearings, misgivings, and misunderstandings in physician‐patient dialogues. Discourse Processes, 7(2), 107–134. https://doi.org/10.1080/01638538409544586
  • West, C. (1990). Not just `doctors’ orders’: Directive-response sequences in patients’ visits to women and men physicians. Discourse & Society, 1(1), 85–112. https://doi.org/10.1177/0957926590001001005
  • West, C. (2006). Coordinating closings in primary care visits: Producing continuity of care. In D. W. Maynard & J. Heritage (Eds.), Communication in medical care: Interaction between primary care physicians and patients (pp. 379–415). Cambridge University Press; Cambridge Core.
  • WHO, & UNICEF. (2018). A vision for primary health care in the 21st century: Towards universal health coverage and the sustainable development goals (Technical Series On Primary Health Care).
  • Wingard, L., Olsher, D., Sabee, C., Vandergriff, I., & Koenig, C. (2014). The blood sugar solicitation sequence: An opening inquiry about blood sugar levels in routine type 2 diabetes visits. In C. M. Jacknick, C. Box, & H. Z. Waring (Eds.), Talk in institutions: A LANSI issue (pp. 74–96). Cambridge Scholars Publishing.
  • Wu, L. (2017). Symptom assessment and patient resistance in primary care interactions in Chinese hospitals. East Asian Pragmatics, 2(2), 259–288. https://doi.org/10.1558/eap.34693