1,528
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Effects of cooperative learning in youth athletics’ motivational climate, peer relationships and self-concept

, , &
Received 06 Jul 2022, Accepted 30 Jun 2023, Published online: 10 Jul 2023

ABSTRACT

Background

Youth sport is threatened by an enormous dropout of prospective athletes. For this reason, we sought a training approach that would enhance young athletes’ learning in areas that should be critical for their retention in sport. According to current literature, a mastery motivational climate, good peer relationships and a good self-concept should have a positive impact on the length of an athlete’s career. The results of previous studies, albeit in physical education (PE), have shown the effectiveness of Cooperative Learning (CL) in achieving various affective, social, physical and cognitive learning goals. However, the impact of CL in competitive sport has not yet been investigated.

Purpose

The aim of this study was to investigate in youth athletics, (a) whether CL is more effective than previously used Direct Instruction (DI) in achieving specific goals in the social and affective domains, and (b) whether CL can promote the development of movement skills at least to the same extent as DI.

Methods

An experimental pretest–posttest comparison group design was used. Twelve Slovenian athletics trainers and 157 athletes (9.56 ± 0.73 years) were included in the sample. They were randomly divided into an experimental group (EG) that completed three consecutive CL units and a control group (CG) that continued with DI according to the control programme. The effects of the models were assessed using three standardised psychological questionnaires measuring peer relationships, motivational climate and self-concept. The movement skills of the young athletes were measured using four standardised fitness tests. One-Way ANCOVAs were conducted to examine whether the model (CL vs. DI) influenced posttest scores, taking into account pretest scores. We also captured all three elements of fidelity to CL.

Findings

There were significant posttest differences between the EG and the CG in peer relationships, emotional self-concept, mastery and performance motivational climate. The differences between the groups on the four physical variables were not significant.

Conclusion

The results confirmed that CL promotes better peer relationships, higher levels of mastery motivational climate, lower levels of performance climate and better emotional self-concept in young track and field athletes compared to DI. According to the current literature, improving these social and affective variables should reduce dropout rates in promising athletes. Therefore, we appeal to athletics federations to recognise the benefits of CL. We propose to inform trainers about the positive effects of CL and to include it in the education and training of trainers.

Introduction

In our country, as in other European countries, youth sport is characterised by a high dropout rate among promising athletes: Slovenia – 85% (Železnik and Škof Citation2020), Finland – 75% (Enoksen Citation2011), Germany and the UK – 50-60% (Vaeyens et al. Citation2009), Belgium – 80% (De Martelaer et al. Citation2004), etc. The reasons for dropping out are too demanding trainings, early specialisation, injuries, burnout, poor relationships with trainers and among athletes, lack of motivation, etc. (Železnik and Škof Citation2020). Moreover, our country is unique in its small size, which means that already the pool of promising athletes is very small. Therefore, it is important to look for possible approaches to training young athletes that could help limit dropout.

Enabling the entire population to participate in sport within their abilities and interests, and increasing the number of athletes who consistently and successfully achieve excellence, are the main goals of the Long-Term Athlete Development (LTAD) model (Way et al. Citation2016). It is one of the modern models of talent development in sport that offer an alternative to early specialisation. There is no empirical evidence so far that one-sided, intensive, goal-oriented training of children in one sport would produce elite athletes. Moreover, the correlation between the sporting success of children under 10 and top sporting performance in the membership category is negative (Güllich and Emrich Citation2014). As talent identification models currently have relatively low predictive capabilities, youth competitive sport should be a place for all. The LTAD model promotes developmentally appropriate physical activity because the authors believe that individuals can only achieve improved physical literacy, athletic excellence and optimal health if their needs are addressed (Way et al. Citation2016). Based on their biological development, the LTAD recommendations include movement and sport skills that athletes need to develop at a certain age. In addition, the LTAD model emphasises that mental, cognitive and emotional factors are also essential for the development of every athlete. Ethics, fair play and character building should be present at all stages. 9-to 11-year-old children are in the Learn to train or major skill learning stage (Way et al. Citation2016). As children are most likely to learn fundamental movement skills in pre-adolescence, this should be encouraged in most training sessions. Pre-adolescence is also a sensitive period of competence development (Way et al. Citation2016). This means that 9-to 11-year-old children already recognise differences in their abilities, and if they see themselves as inferior, they may withdraw from participation in sport. Based on the child’s emotional development, the LTAD model recommends spending only 5-10% of the available time on competition. In fact, competition limits learning opportunities in some areas (Grineski Citation1996). Cooperation, on the other hand, allows children to develop different skills that cannot be achieved through the other goal structures (Hortigüela Alcalá et al. Citation2019). To avoid strenuous training, injuries and burnout, and to reduce dropout rates in sport, the LTAD model is an option for trainers. However, it lacks information on the approach to teaching and learning the listed skills. Although it takes a holistic approach, it puts more emphasis on physical goals than on the psychosocial aspects of children’s behaviour.

In youth sport, it is very important to focus on different learning goals – not only physical, but also affective, social and cognitive (Bailey et al. Citation2009). Cooperative Learning (CL) is a pedagogical model, a teaching and learning approach that enables physical education (PE) students to achieve goals in all four learning domains (Casey and Goodyear Citation2015). The impact of CL in competitive sport has not yet been investigated. Several studies in PE have confirmed the positive impact of CL on social learning (Bjørke and Mordal Moen Citation2020; Guzmán and Payá Citation2020; Hortigüela Alcalá et al. Citation2019). Although the effects of CL on the affective domain have rarely been investigated, some studies have reported improvements in PE students’ motivation and self-concept (Fernandez-Rio et al. Citation2017; Goodyear and Casey Citation2015; Goodyear, Casey, and Kirk Citation2014; Wang Citation2012). However, some researchers (Larsson and Nyberg Citation2017) question the effectiveness of CL in the physical domain, as it may allow for less active time than Direct Instruction (DI) – the approach still most commonly used in practise, where the teacher plays the main role and is the only person imparting knowledge (Guzmán and Payá Citation2020).

In CL, children work in heterogeneous pairs or small groups, cooperating and helping each other to achieve group goals (Cecchini Estrada et al. Citation2019). Learning in small groups allows them to develop their own knowledge while teaching peers (Johnson and Johnson Citation2009). A teacher primarily encourages children to learn from and with each other at their own pace. This type of learning takes into account children’s needs and interests, which is similar to the LTAD model (Casey Citation2017; Way et al. Citation2016). In addition, the cooperative nature of CL prevents the focus of physical activity from being too goal-oriented. A performance motivational climate has been found to reflect negative outcomes such as lack of fun, boredom, lower cohesion and even anxiety (Braithwaite, Spray, and Warburton Citation2011; Ommundsen et al. Citation2005). In contrast, a mastery climate is associated with better mental health (Sheehan, Herring, and Campbell Citation2018). Studies show that motivational climate influences the length of an athlete’s career (Moore and Weiller-Abels Citation2020; Weiss, Moehnke, and Kipp Citation2021).

The idea of integrating CL into youth sport is not about eliminating competition, but creating a cooperative space that would help young athletes improve their relationships with peers and trainers and develop various social skills (Jacobs, Teh, and Spencer Citation2017). Indeed, mastering the latter has a positive impact on success and satisfaction in sport (Jowett and Poczwardowski Citation2007). These two are among the most important motives for athletes to stay in sport. Therefore, if CL is used in youth sport, it could minimise the risk of early specialisation, physical and mental overload, and even burnout. For these reasons, CL could curb dropout in promising athletes and should therefore be tested in youth sport.

The aim of our study was to compare, in youth athletics, the effectiveness of two models – CL and DI (which is commonly used in practise) – in achieving learning goals in the physical, affective and social domains. It was hypothesised that: (a) there will be statistically significant differences between CL and DI in terms of their effects on young track and field athletes’ peer relationships, motivational climate and self-concept; (b) the differences between CL and DI in improving young track and field athletes’ movement skills will not be significant.

Materials and methods

Design

A pretest–posttest research design was used to investigate the effects of an intervention programme with CL and control programme (DI) on group and motivational climate, self-concept and movement skills of children in athletics. An experiment was conducted under non-manipulated (intact) conditions with naturally occurring groups.

Context

The Republic of Slovenia is a small country in central-southeast Europe with a population of 2.052 million. The official language is Slovenian. On the day 31.12.2021, 6534 young athletes born in 2009 were registered at the Olympic Committee of Slovenia – Association of Sports Federations (OCS-ASF Citation2022), of which 388 were registered with the Slovenian Athletic Federation.

Due to the socio-cultural specificities in Slovenia (e.g. low population and number of athletes, relatively low absolute resources for sport development) and the use of the Slovenian language, none of the well-known and psychometrically tested instruments developed manly in Anglo-Saxon cultural contexts could be used without adaptation. Therefore, there are only a few instruments that have been translated and validated into the Slovenian language so that they can be used in the Slovenian socio-cultural sport context.

Participants and procedure

In order to create the largest possible sample, we contacted athletics clubs from all over the country. We included all clubs that met certain criteria, the fulfilment of which made it possible to introduce the intervention in the first place: at least one group of 9-to 11-year-olds; at least twelve children training regularly; possibility of using indoor sports facilities in winter; training accessories; trainer with appropriate education (university degree, pedagogy) or qualification (at least first level and at least seven years of professional experience); trainer is willing to participate in the study; training takes place two or three times a week with the same trainer.

All twelve clubs selected are located in the urban areas of several cities in Slovenia (see Appendix A). By recruiting twelve trainers for the experiment, all their athletes aged 9–11 years were invited to participate in the study. The children were randomly divided into an experimental group (EG) (six clubs, 89 children) and a control group (CG) (six clubs, 68 children). 157 children participated in both measurements and in at least 70% of the training sessions. The EG with CL consisted of 32 girls and 57 boys (mean age: 9.26 ± 0.65 years). The CG with the traditional DI consisted of 39 girls and 29 boys (mean age: 9.89 ± 0.76 years). Further demographic data can be found in Appendix A. However, the procedure is shown in .

Table 1. Timeline.

Model fidelity

In the next subsections, we will try to determine the fidelity of the intervention. Although each pedagogical model has its own idea and set of specific features, each model is flexible and allows practitioners to design units adapted to the specific circumstances of their context (Hastie and Casey Citation2014; Kirk Citation2013). Therefore, we ventured to use CL in the context of Slovenian youth competitive sport. To adequately understand the findings of our study, we needed to accurately describe the circumstances. We reported on all three elements of model fidelity that should be considered when researching educational approaches (Casey, Goodyear, and Dyson Citation2015; Hastie and Casey Citation2014).

A rich description of the curricular elements of the unit

The first author prepared a thorough intervention (and control) programme. The intervention programme began with Introduction to CL, as trainers and children needed to get used to their new roles (Casey, Goodyear, and Dyson Citation2015). The first unit started with cooperation games (icebreakers), which did not yet contain all the key elements of CL. The trainers added them gradually, in line with the programme (see Appendix B). In the second and third unit, the trainers had to form permanent, heterogeneous (by gender, abilities, knowledge, psychosocial characteristics, friendships) groups of four (±1).

The organisation of the learning process was based on cooperative structures that determined how the children worked together and what their learning goals were (Appendix B). Pairs-Check-Perform (Grineski Citation1996; based on Kagan (Citation1992)) was introduced first because learning in pairs is much easier than working in larger groups. Jigsaw (Grineski Citation1996) was also frequently used for learning basic track and field skills. Each member of a jigsaw group was assigned to learn a segment in temporarily formed expert groups. After the children returned to their jigsaw groups, each child was asked to teach their segment to the other group members. Collective Score (Orlick Citation1982) was used primarily to develop movement skills (Kane and Kane Citation2004). Each group member contributed to the team with their score. Group goal was to improve the collective score or to be no worse than given. The main goal of STAD (Slavin Citation1995) was also to help others improve their score to make the most progress as a group but the grading system was more complicated. Groups received medals based on the percentage of improvement over their first attempt. PACER (Barrett Citation2005; Kane and Kane Jr 2004) was used to improve running technique. The pairs were given special flashcards with coordination exercises and had to take on the role of performer and observer. The trainers also assigned specific roles to the children (e.g. performer, trainer, timekeeper, referee, etc.) to work in Learning Teams (Johnson and Johnson Citation1994).

The cooperative structures promoted peer teaching and all five CL non-negotiables. Learning materials were provided to the children so that face-to-face promotive interaction was encouraged. Positive interdependence and individual accountability were promoted as each member of a jigsaw group received only one piece of information needed to complete a group task. Therefore, each child depended on the other group members. The children were assigned different roles so that each team member was responsible for a part of the group task. Since the children had to cooperate to achieve the group goals, positive interdependence was created (Grineski Citation1996). PACER also emphasised positive interdependence, by requiring all members to reach a certain level of competence in coordination exercises before the group (consisting of two pairs) could play a game. Individual accountability was also promoted by publicly presenting both the group's progress and individual results.

As part of the affective goals, the interpersonal and small group skills were defined separately for each training session (Appendix B). The trainers presented each skill to the children and they wrote it together on a special poster that accompanied them throughout the experiment. Group processing was done at the end of each session. It evolved from a whole group discussion led by the trainer to an independent debate in fixed groups.

In the CG, the trainers continued to use DI, i.e. they were the only ones who set tasks, determined the course and pace of learning, assessed the achievement of objectives and monitored the group's interactions (Metzler Citation2011). The work was organised frontally so that the children had the same tasks at the same time. The goal structure was either individual or competitive. The control programme corresponded to the intervention programme in terms of content. Regardless of the used model, the children worked on the same physical goals. However, there were differences in psycho-social learning, while DI does not allow for all types of goal attainment, as is typical for CL.

A detailed validation of model implementation

To determine model fidelity, i.e. whether reported learning outcomes can be attributed to the pedagogical model, we recorded four randomly selected training sessions from each athletics group in the EG (Polvi and Telama Citation2000; Zach, Cohen, and Arnon Citation2020). Data were collected through systematic event coding of the 17 categories of the Cooperative Learning Validation Tool (CLVT) (see Appendix C). It was developed by Dyson (Citation2010) and tested and modified by Casey, Goodyear, and Dyson (Citation2015). Observations were conducted by the first author. In the quantitative analysis of the CLVT data, the average percentages of each coded category were calculated. As part of the process of using the CLVT, qualitative data were collected in the form of Validation Tool Field Notes (VTFN) (Casey, Goodyear, and Dyson Citation2015; Goodyear Citation2017). In addition to the VTFNs, the Post Lesson Teacher Analysis Tool (PLTA) was used to report on children’s learning and the trainers’ actions from the trainers’ perspective (Bodsworth and Goodyear Citation2017). They were asked to write structured reflections after each training session.

The CLVT results showed that we achieved a satisfactory degree of CL model fidelity (Appendix C). All critical elements of CL were used in 75% of the sessions, but group processing was done in all sessions. Other key concepts of CL beyond the five non-negotiables (categories 2–6 in Appendix C) were also observed in about three-quarters of the recorded training sessions. We found that the percentage of observed CL key elements would be even higher if the structures and non-negotiables were not gradually added (Appendix B).

Student learning was assessed in each session and improvements were made in 92% of the sessions, reflecting high student engagement (Appendix C). The number of learning assessments and improvements observed was highest in the social or emotional domain. At the end of each training session children were asked to reflect in writing on the psychosocial aspects of their own behaviour, the behaviour of their peers and the behaviour of the trainer (VTFN). The CLVT revealed that social/emotional and cognitive goals were observed in 75% of the recorded sessions (Appendix C). However, physical goals were observed in every training session. Consistent with the CLVT findings, trainers reported improvements mainly in the social or emotional and physical domains (PLTA).

We cannot claim that full fidelity was achieved in every session, and there are certainly examples highlighted by CLVT, VTFN and PLTA that show variations in the degree of fidelity achieved. However, this moderate to high degree of model fidelity allows us to assume that children’s response to the units was the result of CL (Bjørke and Mordal Moen Citation2020; Casey, Goodyear, and Dyson Citation2015).

A detailed description of the programme context that includes the previous experiences of the trainer and children with the model

All trainers (except me – the first author) had used only the traditional approach (DI) before the study. I had discovered CL about a year before the study. It caught my attention so I first did a literature review and then started using it in practise as I work as an athletic trainer for children. The impact of CL on children’s learning is also the topic of my PhD thesis (in progress). I decided to train one of the groups in the EG to get a deep insight into the implementation of the intervention. I have also conducted a coach training for CL for the trainers in the EG. We met five times from March to October and conducted two lectures and three workshops, which lasted a total of 20 h (). In the first lecture, the trainers received explanations about goal structures, learning objectives, the history of CL, pedagogical models in general, basic features of CL and findings from previous studies on CL. In the second lecture, the non-negotiables and structures of CL were theoretically presented to the trainers. They were also given a first insight into the intervention programme. During the workshops, the trainers learned about general cooperative activities as well as specific cooperative structures with an athletics content. All workshops were recorded so the participants received video material. To check whether learning had taken place, they were given the task to choose a cooperative structure (from the programme) and a track and field discipline and present it practically in the third workshop. Other trainers had the role of performers (during the activities) and CL experts (after the presentations, during the group processing). During the experiment we were in constant contact with the trainers. We met regularly remotely and communicated by phone and email to solve various dilemmas, deepen the trainers’ knowledge of CL and adapt the plan according to the circumstances.

Details of the participating children can be found in the subchapter Participants and Procedure and in Appendix A. They had no previous experience with CL.

Data collection

As described above, quantitative and qualitative data were collected with CLVT, VTFN and PLTA to report on fidelity to CL. All other data collected was quantitative. Flamingo, flying sprint, T-test and curl-up were used to measure children’s movement skills or to report on the achievement of physical goals. Social and emotional improvements, on the other hand, were determined using three standardised psychological questionnaires to measure peer relationships, self-concept and motivational climate.

Movement skills

Standardised fitness tests were used to measure four movement skills, which were defined as the most important according to the children’s biological development (Way et al. Citation2016). The flamingo balance test (Sember, Grošelj, and Pajek Citation2020) requires standing on one leg on a special beam while the other leg is bent at the knee and the foot of this leg is held close to the buttocks. The 15-metre flying sprint measures speed (Mackala et al. Citation2019). The t-test (Pauloe et al. Citation2000) is a simple running test of agility that includes forward, sideways and backward movements and is suitable for a variety of sports. The curl-up abdominal test (Allen et al. Citation2014) requires you to perform as many curl-ups as possible with a cadence of one every three seconds.

Peer relationships

It was assessed using the My Class questionnaire (Fisher and Fraser Citation1981). Validation including factor analysis and internal consistency analysis for Slovenian conditions (Zabukovec Citation1998; N = 134, 9–10 years old) confirmed the psychometric properties of My Class. The questionnaire consists of 25 dichotomous questions (items) representing two factors – personal development (e.g. ‘We argue a lot in our athletics group.’) and peer relationships (e.g. ‘Every member of our athletics group is my friend.’). The Slovenian version of My Class was modified for the purposes of this study. The changes only concerned terminology (e.g. ‘class’ was replaced by ‘athletics group’). Reliability (α(personal development) = 0.70, α(peer relationships) = 0.74) was similar to the original and Slovenian (Zabukovec Citation1998) versions of My Class.

Motivational climate

The Motivational climate was measured with the Learning and Performance Orientations in PE Classes Questionnaire (LAPOPECQ; Papaioannou Citation1994). A pilot study of the Slovenian version (Cecić Erpič et al. Citation2004) confirmed that all items had satisfactory psychometric properties and confirmatory factor analysis showed that the items could be assigned to the same factors as in the original questionnaire. The Slovenian version consists of 25 items answered on a five-point Likert-type scale (1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree). The items represent five factors: (1) the teacher/trainer behaviour (e.g. ‘The trainer is very satisfied when each child learns something new.’); (2) the children’s learning orientation (e.g. ‘We – the children – are most satisfied when we learn a new motor skill.’); (3) the children’s competitive orientation (e.g. ‘We – the children – are most satisfied when we are better than others.’); (4) outcome orientation without effort (e.g. ‘It is very important to win without trying hard.’); (5) children’s worries about mistakes (e.g. ‘We – the children – feel bad when we make mistakes.’). The first two factors represent the variable ‘mastery motivational climate’. The second variable ‘performance motivational climate’ is composed of factors 3-5. Some terminological changes had to be made in order to use the questionnaire in the context of competitive sport (e.g. ‘teacher’ was replaced by ‘trainer’). The Cronbach’s alphas obtained were: F1 = 0.69, F2 = 0.74, F3 = 0.82, F4 = 0.74, F5 = 0.69. Reliability was similar to the original and Slovenian versions of the LAPOPECQ.

Self-concept

It was assessed using three subscales of the Self-Concept Questionnaire (SCQ). The original version is Autoconcepto Forma A (AFA; Musitu, García, and Gutiérrez Citation1994). It was validated for the Slovenian context by Kranjc in 1997 (Musitu et al. Citation1998). However, in 2018, Gosar rechecked the psychometric characteristics (N = 266, 9–11 years old) and confirmed the validity and reliability of the Slovenian version of the SCQ (Musitu et al. Citation2021). The three subscales measured were: social (e.g. ‘I like helping other children in the group.’), physical (e.g. ‘I am satisfied with my appearance.’) and emotional (e.g. ‘I get nervous when the trainer calls me.’). They consist of 27 items. The item response is a 3-point Likert-type scale that measures frequency. The Cronbach’s alphas obtained correspond to those of Musitu et al. (Citation1998): social – 0.75, physical – 0.80, emotional – 0.73.

Data analysis

All data were analysed using IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, version 26. Descriptive statistics were first compiled and pretest differences between groups were tested (see ). Exploratory analyses were then conducted to determine whether the data met the assumptions for analysis of covariance (ANCOVA). To examine whether posttest scores differed between the models (CL vs. DI), One-Way ANCOVAs were used. The pretest scores were added as covariates. Finally, effect sizes were calculated and reported. For all statistical analyses, the significance level was set at p ≤ 0.05.

Table 2. Intragroup changes and between groups differences at pretest.

Results

shows the descriptive statistics and between-group differences at pretest. Significant differences and medium effect sizes in favour of the CG were found for personal development, peer relationships, flamingo, 15 m flying and t-test. Non-significant differences and small effect sizes (which can be considered trivial) were found for other dependent variables.

To compare the effects of CL and DI on motivational climate, peer relationships, self-concept and movement skills, and to control for pretest scores on the dependent variables, we used ANCOVAs for further analysis. Post-test, there were significant differences in peer relationships, emotional self-concept and motivational climate between the EG and the CG (see ). CL proved to be more effective than DI in improving peer relationships and emotional self-concept in young track and field athletes. The EG improved mastery motivational climate and decreased performance climate to a greater extent than the CG. At posttest, children in the CG actually perceived the mastery climate to be slightly lower than at pretest (). However, the effect sizes were small for all outcome variables (). The main results of the study were related to the psychosocial aspects of the young athletes’ behaviour. Nevertheless, the children’s movement skills were also measured and compared between groups to confirm that the learning process was also focused on physical goals, both in the CG and in the EG. This is because a learning approach that does not take into account the development of movement skills would be useless for youth competitive sport. The differences between the groups on the four variables representing children’s movement skills were not significant ().

Table 3. ANCOVA for posttest differences between EG and CG in group climate, self-concept and motivational climate.

Table 4. ANCOVA for posttest differences between EG and CG in movement skills.

Discussion

The results confirmed a greater positive impact of CL compared to DI on children’s affective (self-concept and motivation) and social learning (peer relationships) (see ), which is consistent with the findings of previous research in PE (Casey and Goodyear Citation2015). Peer relationships and mastery motivational climate improved only in the EG () and the differences between the two groups at posttest were significant (). Bjørke and Mordal Moen (Citation2020) also confirmed the positive effects of CL on peer relationships. Hortigüela Alcalá et al. (Citation2019) found that implementing CL and teaching social skills to pre-adolescent children can help them become less eccentric. Social skills such as mutual trust, respect and support, understanding and cooperation are fundamental components of a good relationship (Hortigüela Alcalá et al. Citation2019; Jowett and Poczwardowski Citation2007). Although athletics is an individual sport, good relationships between participants are very important (Lisinskiene Citation2018). Developing good relationships with peers and trainers allows athletes to enjoy sporting activities, develop higher motivation, be more successful and continue to participate in sport (Jacobs, Teh, and Spencer Citation2017). An important contribution of our study to the current literature is also the finding that CL can improve the motivational climate in youth sport. Improving the mastery climate and lowering the performance climate can improve athletes’ mental health (Sheehan, Herring, and Campbell Citation2018) and increase the likelihood that they will stay in sport longer (Moore and Weiller-Abels Citation2020; Weiss, Moehnke, and Kipp Citation2021). Poor mental health (e. g. severe mood swings, depression, poor sleep quality, anxiety, etc.) and dropout can also be the result of poor self-concept in athletes (Lee Citation2020). Therefore, we have investigated and successfully demonstrated the greater positive effects of CL compared to DI on the emotional self-concept of young athletes.

Considering the philosophy of modern athlete development models in sport (including the theoretical background of the LTAD model), CL could be an appropriate training approach for children. As it can improve peer relationships, motivational climate and self-concept of young athletes, it could also help to improve the mental health of young athletes and reduce the dropout rate in youth sports. Therefore, it is very important for trainers to think about the type of teaching approach they use and how it affects the psychosocial aspects of children’s behaviour that are crucial for their retention in sport.

In PE and youth sport, psychosocial and cognitive goals are gaining importance, but the main effects of the learning process are still physical (Bailey et al. Citation2009). As this is a definition of quality physical activity for children, every teacher/trainer should ask themselves whether their choice of teaching approach enables as many children as possible to achieve as many learning goals as possible (Grineski Citation1996). With CL, all four learning goals of PE can be achieved (Casey and Goodyear Citation2015). Our study contributed to the science the finding that at least two (affective and social) learning domains in youth sport can be achieved with CL and that goal achievement in these two domains is more effective with CL compared to DI. The children in the EG also improved in the physical domain (), but the differences between the EG and the CG in movement skills were not statistically significant (). Nevertheless, the second hypothesis of our study was confirmed, although some researchers believe that CL allows less active time than DI and consequently lower improvements in movement and sport skills (Larsson and Nyberg Citation2017).

Conclusion

Given the huge dropout of young perspective athletes, we sought a pedagogical model that could improve children’s learning in areas that are crucial for young athletes to stay in sport. CL proved to be more effective than DI in improving the motivational climate, peer relationships and emotional self-concept of track and field athletes. Therefore, we propose to the Athletic federation of Slovenia and also to other national sports federations to establish a systematic coach training for CL so that (future) trainers get a deep insight into this pedagogical model in the context of youth sport. The goal is for trainers to use CL at least in combination with their current teaching approach.

However, we are aware of some limitations of the study. Considering the small number of young track and field athletes in Slovenia, we had to choose randomisation by natural groups. Due to the specific context, we also could not use the matched groups technique for randomisation. Another limitation could be the very different training conditions of the clubs (see Appendix A), which made the implementation of the training programmes difficult. One limitation was certainly the coronavirus pandemic and the associated closures. As a result, the average attendance of children was much lower, so we had to lower the standard for exclusion from the study (normally it is 80%). There were two limitations in determining model fidelity. If we were to study CL another time, we would record all training sessions to get a more realistic picture of model implementation. Also, we should involve another CL expert who would be able to assess model fidelity using the CLVT, but such experts do not exist in Slovenia.

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).

References

  • Allen, B. A., J. C. Hannon, R. D. Burns, and S. M. Williams. 2014. “Effect of a Core Conditioning Intervention on Tests of Trunk Muscular Endurance in School-Aged Children.” Journal of Strength and Conditioning Research 28 (7): 2063–2070. https://doi.org/10.1519/JSC.0000000000000352.
  • Bailey, R., K. Armour, D. Kirk, M. Jess, I. Pickup, and R. Sandford. 2009. “The Educational Benefits Claimed for Physical Education and School Sport: An Academic Review.” Research Papers in Education 24 (1): 1–27. https://doi.org/10.1080/02671520701809817.
  • Barrett, T. 2005. “Effects of Cooperative Learning on the Performance of Sixth-Grade Physical Education Students.” Journal of Teaching in Physical Education 24 (1): 88–102. https://doi.org/10.1123/jtpe.24.1.88.
  • Bjørke, L., and K. Mordal Moen. 2020. “Cooperative Learning in Physical Education: A Study of Students’ Learning Journey Over 24 Lessons.” Physical Education and Sport Pedagogy 25 (6): 600–612. https://doi.org/10.1080/17408989.2020.1761955.
  • Bodsworth, H., and V. A. Goodyear. 2017. “Barriers and Facilitators to Using Digital Technologies in the Cooperative Learning Model in Physical Education.” Physical Education and Sport Pedagogy 22 (6): 563–579. https://doi.org/10.1080/17408989.2017.1294672.
  • Braithwaite, R., C. M. Spray, and V. E. Warburton. 2011. “Motivational Climate Interventions in Physical Education: A Meta-Analysis.” Psychology of Sport and Exercise 12 (6): 628–638. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2011.06.005.
  • Casey, A. 2017. “Models-Based Practice.” In In Routledge Handbook of Physical Education Pedagogies, edited by C. D. Ennis, 54–67. London: Routledge.
  • Casey, A., and V. A. Goodyear. 2015. “Can Cooperative Learning Achieve the Four Learning Outcomes of Physical Education? A Review of Literature.” Quest (grand Rapids, Mich ) 67 (1): 56–72. https://doi.org/10.1080/00336297.2014.984733.
  • Casey, A., V. A. Goodyear, and B. Dyson. 2015. “Model Fidelity and Students’ Responses to an Authenticated Unit of Cooperative Learning.” Journal of Teaching in Physical Education 34 (4): 642–660. https://doi.org/10.1123/jtpe.2013-0227.
  • Cecchini Estrada, J. A., C. González González-Mesa, R. Llamedo, B. Sánchez Martínez, and C. Rodríguez Pérez. 2019. “The Impact of Cooperative Learning on Peer Relationships, Intrinsic Motivation and Future Intentions to Do Sport.” Psicothema 31 (2): 163–169. https://doi.org/10.7334/psicothema2018.305.
  • Cecić Erpič, S., D. Boben, B. Škof, V. Novak Zabukovec, and R. Barić. 2004. “Psychometric Properties of the Motivational Climate Inventory in Physical Education: A Confirmatory Factor Analysis.” Kinesiologia Slovenica 10 (2): 16–25.
  • De Martelaer, K., R. Rzewnicki, P. De Knop, and P. Wylleman. 2004. “Parents and Trainers: A Help or Harm? Affective Outcomes for Children in Sport.” In Ethics in Youth Sport, edited by Y. V. Auweele, 179–193. Leuven: Lanoocampus.
  • Dyson, B. 2010. “The Co-Construction of Cooperative Learning as a Pedagogical Strategy in New Zealand Physical Education Programs.” In Paper Session Presented at the Australian Association of Research on Education, Melbourne, Australia.
  • Enoksen, E. 2011. “Drop-out Rate and Drop-out Reasons among Promising Norwegian Track and Field Athletes.” Scandinavian Sport Studies Forum 2: 19–43.
  • Fernandez-Rio, J., N. Sanz, J. Fernandez-Cando, and L. Santos. 2017. “Impact of a Sustained Cooperative Learning Intervention on Student Motivation.” Physical Education and Sport Pedagogy 22 (1): 89–105. https://doi.org/10.1080/17408989.2015.1123238.
  • Fisher, D. L., and B. J. Fraser. 1981. “Validity and Use of My Class Inventory.” Science Education 65: 145–156. https://doi.org/10.1002/sce.3730650206.
  • Goodyear, V. A. 2017. “Sustained Professional Development on Cooperative Learning: Impact on Six Teachers’ Practices and Students’ Learning.” Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport 88 (1): 83–94. https://doi.org/10.1080/02701367.2016.1263381.
  • Goodyear, V. A., and A. Casey. 2015. “Innovation with Change: Developing a Community of Practice to Help Teachers Move Beyond the ‘Honeymoon’ of Pedagogical Renovation.” Physical Education and Sport Pedagogy 20 (2): 186–203. https://doi.org/10.1080/17408989.2013.817012.
  • Goodyear, V. A., A. Casey, and D. Kirk. 2014. “Hiding Behind the Camera: Social Learning Within the Cooperative Learning Model to Engage Girls in Physical Education.” Sport, Education and Society 18 (6): 712–734. https://doi.org/10.1080/13573322.2012.707124.
  • Grineski, S. 1996. Cooperative Learning in Physical Education. Champaign: Human Kinetics.
  • Guzmán, J. F., and E. Payá. 2020. “Direct Instruction vs. Cooperative Learning in Physical Education: Effects on Student Learning, Behaviors, and Subjective Experience.” Sustainability 12 (12): 48–93. https://doi.org/10.3390/SU12124893.
  • Güllich, A., and E. Emrich. 2014. “Considering Long-Term Sustainability in the Development of World-Class Success.” European Journal for Sport and Society 14 (1): 383–397. https://doi.org/10.1080/17461391.2012.706320.
  • Hastie, P. A., and A. Casey. 2014. “Fidelity in Models-Based Practice Research in Sport Pedagogy: A Guide for Future Investigations.” Journal of Teaching in Physical Education 33 (3): 422–431. https://doi.org/10.1123/jtpe.2013-0141.
  • Hortigüela Alcalá, D., A. Hernando Garijo, Á Pérez-Pueyo, and J. Fernández-Río. 2019. “Cooperative Learning and Students’ Motivation, Social Interactions and Attitudes: Perspectives from Two Different Educational Stages.” Sustainability 11 (24): 1–11. https://doi.org/10.3390/su11247005.
  • Jacobs, G. M., J. Teh, and L. Spencer. 2017. “A Proposal for Facilitating More Cooperation in Competitive Sports.” Journal of Chemical Information and Modeling 53 (9): 1689–1699.
  • Johnson, D. W., and R. T. Johnson. 1994. Learning Together and Alone. Cooperative, Competitive, and Individualistic Learning. 4th ed. Edina, MN: Interaction Book Company.
  • Johnson, D. W., and R. T. Johnson. 2009. “An Educational Psychology Success Story: Social Interdependence Theory and Cooperative Learning.” Educational Researcher 38 (5): 365–379. https://doi.org/10.3102/0013189X09339057.
  • Jowett, S., and A. Poczwardowski. 2007. “Understanding the Trainer-Athlete Relationship.” In Social Psychology in Sport, edited by S. Jowett, and D. Lavalee, 3–14. Human Kinetics.
  • Kagan, S. 1992. Cooperative Learning. San Clemente, CA: Kagan Cooperative Learning.
  • Kane, J. J., and R. J. Kane Jr. 2004. “Using Cooperative Learning Strategies to Teach Children Fitness.” Teaching Elementary Physical Education 15 (1): 24–27.
  • Kirk, D. 2013. “Educational Value and Models-Based Practice in Physical Education.” Educational Philosophy and Theory 45 (9): 973–986. https://doi.org/10.1080/00131857.2013.785352.
  • Larsson, H., and G. Nyberg. 2017. “‘It Doesn’t Matter how They Move Really, as Long as They Move.’ Physical Education Teachers on Developing Their Students’ Movement Capabilities.” Physical Education and Sport Pedagogy 22 (2): 137–149. https://doi.org/10.1080/17408989.2016.1157573.
  • Lee, K. 2020. “Social Support and Self-Esteem on the Association Between Stressful Life Events and Mental Health Outcomes among College Students.” Social Work in Health Care 59 (6): 387–407. https://doi.org/10.1080/00981389.2020.1772443.
  • Lisinskiene, A. 2018. “The Effect of a 6-Month Trainer Educational Program on Strengthening Trainer-Athlete Interpersonal Relationships in Individual Youth Sport.” Sports 6 (3): 74. https://doi.org/10.3390/sports6030074.
  • Mackala, K., M. Fostiak, B. Schweyen, T. Osik, and M. Čoh. 2019. “Acute Effects of a Speed Training Program on Sprinting Step Kinematics and Performance.” International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 16 (17): 31–38. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph16173138.
  • Metzler, M. W. 2011. Instructional Models for Physical Education. New York: Routledge.
  • Moore, E. W. G., and K. Weiller-Abels. 2020. “Psychosocial Climates Differentially Predict 12-to 14-Year-Old Competitive Soccer Players’ Goal Orientations.” Women in Sport and Physical Activity Journal 28 (2): 111–118. https://doi.org/10.1123/wspaj.2019-0007.
  • Musitu, G., F. García, and M. Gutiérrez. 1994. AFA: Autoconcepto Forma A. Madrid: TEA Ediciones, S.A.
  • Musitu, G., F. García, M. Gutiérrez, I. Krajnc, S. Pečjak, D. Boben, and M. Bajt. 1998. Vprašalnik Samopodobe: Spa. Priročnik [The Origin of the Self-Concept Questionnaire: Scq. Manual]. Ljubljana: Center za psihodiagnostična sredstva.
  • Musitu, G., F. García, M. Gutiérrez, I. Krajnc, S. Pečjak, and D. Gosar. 2021. Vprašalnik Samopodobe: Spa. Priročnik [The Origin of the Self-Concept Questionnaire: Scq. Manual]. Ljubljana: Center za psihodiagnostična sredstva.
  • Olympic Committee of Slovenia – Association of Sports Federations. 2022. “Arhivska Evidenca Registriranih športnikov [The Origin of the Archival Evidence of Registered Athletes].” Olympic, January 1. https://www.olympic.si/sportniki/registracija-in-kategorizacija.
  • Ommundsen, Y., G. Roberts, P. Lemrye, and P. W. Miller. 2005. “Peer Relationships in Adolescent Competitive Soccer: Associations to Perceived Motivational Climate, Achievement Goals, and Perfectionism.” Journal of Sports Sciences 23 (9): 977–989. https://doi.org/10.1080/02640410500127975.
  • Orlick, T. 1982. The Second Cooperative Sports and Games Book. New York: Pantheon.
  • Papaioannou, A. 1994. “Development of a Questionnaire to Measure Achievement Orientations in Physical Education.” Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport 65 (1): 11–20. https://doi.org/10.1080/02701367.1994.10762203.
  • Pauloe, K., K. Madole, J. Garhammer, M. Lacourse, and R. Rozenek. 2000. “Reliability and Validity of the T-Test as a Measure of Agility, Leg Power, and Leg Speed in College-Aged Men and Women.” Journal of Strength and Conditioning Research 14 (4): 443–450. https://doi.org/10.1519/00124278-200011000-00012.
  • Polvi, S., and R. Telama. 2000. “The Use of Cooperative Learning as a Social Enhancer in Physical Education.” Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research 44 (1): 105–115. https://doi.org/10.1080/713696660.
  • Sember, V., J. Grošelj, and M. Pajek. 2020. “Balance Tests in Pre-Adolescent Children: Retest Reliability, Construct Validity, and Relative Ability.” International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 17 (15): 54–74. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17155474.
  • Sheehan, R. B., M. P. Herring, and M. J. Campbell. 2018. “Associations Between Motivation and Mental Health in Sport: A Test of the Hierarchical Model of Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation.” Frontiers in Psychology 9: 707. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00707.
  • Slavin, R. E. 1995. Cooperative Learning: Theory, Research, and Practice. 2nd ed. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
  • Vaeyens, R., A. Güllich, C. R. Warr, and R. Philippaerts. 2009. “Talent Identification and Promotion Programmes of Olympic Athletes.” Journal of Sports Sciences 27 (13): 1367–1380. https://doi.org/10.1080/02640410903110974.
  • Wang, M. 2012. “Effects of Cooperative Learning on Achievement Motivation of Female University Students.” Asian Social Science 8 (15), https://doi.org/10.5539/ass.v8n15p108.
  • Way, R., C. Trono, D. Mitchell, T. Laing, M. Vahi, C. Meadows, and A. Lau. 2016. “Sport for Life - Long-Term Athlete Development Resource Paper 2.1.” Sport for Life Society. https://proxy.lib.ohio-state.edu/login?url = http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct = true&db = s3h&AN = 23787650&site = ehost-live.
  • Weiss, M. R., H. J. Moehnke, and L. E. Kipp. 2021. “A United Front: Trainer and Teammate Motivational Climate and Team Cohesion among Female Adolescent Athletes.” International Journal of Sports Science & Coaching 16 (4): 875–885. https://doi.org/10.1177/17479541211006905.
  • Zabukovec, V. 1998. Merjenje Razredne Klime: Priročnik za Učitelje [The Origin of the Measuring Class Climate: Manual for Teachers]. Ljubljana: Productivity, Psycho Diagnostic Center.
  • Zach, S., R. Cohen, and M. Arnon. 2020. “Motivational Climate in Physical Education Classes: Is It Really Determined by the Instructional Model?” The Physical Educator 77 (2): 426–446. https://doi.org/10.18666/TPE-2020-V77-I2-9855.
  • Železnik, L., and B. Škof. 2020. “Dropout Rate of Slovenian’s Most Successful Young Athletes.” Journal of Physical Education and Sport 20 (3): 2182–2188. https://doi.org/10.7752/jpes.2020.s3293.

Appendices

Appendix A

Table A1. Demographic information of the clubs, coaches and youth participants.

Appendix C

Table C1. Results of the systematic event coding on CLVT.