2,647
Views
1
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

The effects of focused ultrasound pulsation of nucleus accumbens in opioid-dependent rats

ORCID Icon, , ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, , ORCID Icon, , , ORCID Icon & show all
Pages 748-759 | Received 28 Feb 2019, Accepted 11 Jun 2019, Published online: 09 Jul 2019

ABSTRACT

Background: Deep Brain Stimulation (DBS) is the only modality proven to be effective on selective stimulation of the deep brain structures. It was previously reported that, by using DBS, stimulation of nucleus accumbens (NA), a region that plays a pivotal role in the pathogenesis of substance addiction, is effective for the treatment of substance addiction.

Objective: The purpose of the current study was to observe how the morphine-conditioned place preference changed in rats by stimulating NA with a non-invasive method, focused ultrasound (US) and to detect whether there would be any tissue damage caused by US waves.

Methods: We used low-intensity focused ultrasound (LIFU), a noninvasive modality, in a place conditioning model to stimulate NA in rats.

Results: At the initial stage of our study, we used morphine to induce place preference. As expected, morphine administration caused significant place preference. After the place preference was obtained by morphine, we divided the rats into two groups. One group received LIFU waves to NA and the other group received only sham, that is, no stimulation with US waves. Rats in both groups were continued to receive morphine. Then, we investigated whether LIFU and sham will reduce morphine-induced place preference or not. We observed that morphine-induced place preference had an ongoing raise in the sham group while no raise was detected in the ultrasound group. Although LIFU prevented the rats from the raise, it did not cause a significant reduction of morphine preference.

Conclusion: We state that there is a need for future studies to investigate the effects of low-intensity focused ultrasound as an alternative treatment modality in addiction.

Introduction

Opioid addiction, which is a type of substance dependence, has been defined by American Society of Addiction Medicine as “being extremely preoccupied in mind with opioid intake despite sufficient level of analgesia is obtained, and loss of control on opioid use” [Citation1]. Mesocorticolimbic dopamine reward pathway, which consists of Ventral Tegmental Area (VTA), Nucleus Accumbens (NA) and Ventral Pallidum (VP), plays a critical role in substance dependence [Citation2,Citation3].

Neuromodulation is defined by The International Neuromodulation Society as “changing nerve activity by giving electrical stimulation or chemical substances to target areas in the body,” which is performed for normalizing and modulating nerve function. Vagal stimulation, Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation (TMS), Transcranial Electrical Stimulation (tES), Deep Brain Stimulation (DBS), High Intensity Focused Ultrasound (HIFU) and Low Intensity Focused Ultrasound (LIFU) can be listed in main neuromodulation methods.

Due to its deep location in the brain, NA cannot be specifically stimulated by noninvasive techniques such as TMS. For this reason, the DBS technique seems to be suitable for NA stimulation. Literature review on these studies revealed that DBS was first applied to NA on 2007 for the treatment of a patient with treatment-resistant panic disorder and had no effect on panic disorder, however, comorbid alcohol dependence was reported to be remitted [Citation4]. High-frequency stimulation of NA with DBS was shown to be effective for lowering Conditioned Place Preference (CPP) test scores stimulated by morphine in rats [Citation5–7]. But DBS is an invasive process that involves surgically placing an electrode deep in the brain. It has risks and complications regarding surgery, stimulation and stimulation pathway [Citation8].

In recent years, ultrasound energy focused on deep brain structures at low energy levels has been shown to stimulate or inhibit small brain areas without causing neuronal damage [Citation9–11]. There has been a remarkable increase in studies concerning neuromodulator features of LIFU waves. A study has shown that, short term ultrasound (US) stimulations on peripheral nerves activate and long term stimulations inhibit amplitude and speed of the action potential [Citation12]. US is an energy type that has a waveform, which is considered to generate a neuromodulator effect due to mechanic vibration [Citation13]. Low intensity US neuromodulation, spreading as short pulses, was suggested to reduce energy storage on tissues for a specific time, and therefore may cause action potentials with mechanical effect by altering neuronal transmission [Citation2,Citation14].

The safety and efficacy of LIFU waves have been demonstrated in many studies performed with animals and the results of the first study on humans have been reported in 2014. In the relevant study performed with ten volunteers, the effect of focused ultrasound on two-point discrimination has been investigated and it was shown to increase somatosensory discrimination capacity in humans [Citation15].

Although DBS has a very good spatial localization capacity, its usage is limited since it is an invasive method. TMS cannot reach deep tissues since it has a superficial effect and it cannot create a target-specific effect since its area of influence is relatively wide. Thus, an ideal neuromodulation method is expected to reach deep structures, to be specific for its target and to be noninvasive. There is no existing method that fully meets these criteria. Animal studies performed so far revealed that focused ultrasound can specifically stimulate and inhibit deep brain tissues without causing harmful effects outside of target point. This method is considered to become a significant step in the treatment of psychiatric and neurological disorders with neuromodulation since it is noninvasive and does not require surgery and anaesthesia. Thus, focused ultrasound is argued to be an alternative method in the treatment of psychiatric and neurological disorders such as mood disorders, schizophrenia, depression, anxiety disorders, epilepsy, traumatic brain injuries and Parkinson's disease in the future.

Our study has been designed on a dependency animal model, one of the models that are considered to reflect human psychiatric disorders best. We suggest that focused ultrasound may open a new field in the treatment of opioid and other substance dependencies in humans and we decided to use rats since those dependencies of rats have similar pathophysiology with humans. In trials concerning substance dependency, various experimental animal models were established based on substance seeking behaviour and withdrawal symptoms of experimental animals. One of these models, CPP, is known to be a valid model for assessment of reinforcing properties of addictive substances, and also scanning for abuse [Citation16]. CPP test is a learning method that provides cooperation between reward and specific brain areas [Citation17]. With the CPP test, it has been determined that experimental animals spend more time in areas which are peered with morphine, ethanol, encephalin, amphetamine and similar drugs and substances compared to the areas which are peered with physiological saline [Citation18]

In the current study, our purpose was to determine the effects of stimulation of NA with a non-invasive method, focused ultrasound, on morphine-dependent place preference with a conditioned place preference model in rats. Our hypothesis in this CPP experimental model study was that LIFU focused on NA would reduce morphine dependency, without causing any tissue damage, via significantly effecting the time spent by rats in morphine-peered section.

Material and method

Selection of animals and sheltering environment

Our study has been conducted in Bezmialem Foundation University Hospital Psychiatry Clinics. Approval of the local ethics committee was taken as required (approval date: 29.01.2015; approval number: 2015/44). 26 adult Sprague-Dawley male rats, weighing between 300–490 g, were used. The details of the experiment environment are presented in the online supplement file 1 (suppl 1).

CPP testing apparatus consisted of two Plexiglas boxes in square and rectangular shape and equal size, separated by a sliding (guillotine) door that can be lifted. In these two chambers of the same size, there were different clues (stimulants) such as the shape of the floor and the colour of the walls that the animals can discriminate (suppl 2).

Steps of experimental addiction model and surgical procedure

Building an addiction model

In numerous previous studies, intraperitoneal morphine injection method was used for building addiction models in rats [Citation19,Citation20]. Likely in the current study, morphine hydrochloride dissolved in normal saline was administered via intraperitoneal (i.p.) route in equal volumes (0.5–1 ml).

Among the 26 rats included in the study, 18 were randomized to morphine group and the resting 8 were randomized to physiologic saline group. CPP test consisted of a three-stage and 14-day calendar. These stages were, before conditioning (pre-conditioning), conditioning and after conditioning (post-conditioning). Pre-conditioning stage lasted for 3 days and it was applied to determine the place preference of animals. Each animal from all groups was placed every day in the central area with sliding guillotine door that allows transport, for about 900 s. On the third day, time spent by rats in each compartment was recorded with the camera (). As seen on , rats spent more time in the black section. This finding is quite likely based upon the need to hide from potential threats. For this reason, a method suitable for the biased design was selected during conditioning. Black section of CPP apparatus was matched with normal saline, while the white section was matched with morphine.

Table 1. Conditioned place preference study progress.

Table 2. Time spent by rats in all groups within white and black compartments in preliminary test stage.

On the first day of the conditioning stage (4th day), normal saline was injected to rats in both groups and they were immediately placed to the black compartment matched with normal saline from 8:00 to 12:00.

After morphine injection to morphine group and normal saline injection to saline group was performed, rats were placed to white compartment matched with morphine from 14:00 to 18:00. On the second day, procedure was performed in reverse order, that is, after performing morphine injection to morphine group and normal saline injection to physiologic saline group, rats were placed to white compartment matched with morphine from 8:00 to 12:00 and then normal saline was injected to rats in both groups, and they were placed in black compartment matched with normal saline from 14:00 to 18:00 (). This procedure continued until day 13. On the last 5 days of conditioning stage (days 9–13), animals from all groups were put on stabilization apparatus for 10 min in the period between two injections (12:00–14:00) and they were accustomed to waiting (). Morphine group received normal saline and morphine hydrochloride injections via i.p. route in 6-hour intervals throughout conditioning stage. Control group received only normal saline injections via i.p. route twice a day throughout the conditioning stage. Doses in preliminary likely studies were determined between 5–50 mg/kg [Citation21,Citation22]. In the current study, throughout conditioning session, 10 mg/kg morphine hydrochloride was administered for 7 days via i.p. route, then it was stabilized at 20 mg/kg dose once a day. Guillotine doors were closed throughout conditioning stage.

Placing the magnets

On day 14, in order to determine the size of the brain area (NA) of LIFU wave administration, data outlined in were used. Roughly, a volume sized 1.3 mm length in X axis, 2 mm width in Y axis and 2.1 mm depth in Z axis was determined to be the optimum geometrical volume for NA.

Figure 1. Stereotaxic imaging of nucleus accumbens in coronal sections of rat brain (taken with permission from The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates, 7th ed., Paxinos and Watson., 2014).

Figure 1. Stereotaxic imaging of nucleus accumbens in coronal sections of rat brain (taken with permission from The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates, 7th ed., Paxinos and Watson., 2014).

Two symmetrical points at 1.85 mm distance from bregma to nasal side, in 7 mm depth from bregma level and at 1.55 mm lateral from middle line at both right and left sides, were determined as middle focus points of sonication (for right and left NA) (, suppl 3).

Figure 2. View of rat scalp; ultrasound focus points are shown in red shapes.

Figure 2. View of rat scalp; ultrasound focus points are shown in red shapes.

Transducers, designed with dimensions compatibly matching to the rat skull, produced ultrasound waves. In order to determine the focus of waves before sonication, a separate study was performed with a bullet hydrophone and a setting of three-dimensional scanning device for measuring the US wave dynamics from behind the rat skull. The data obtained from three-dimensional scanning is presented in and . We determined the permeability of rat skull and focused US wave dynamics by scanning the rat skull spatially with a computerized XYZ scanner and by driving the transducer focused to computerized signal source and measuring the intensity of US with hydrophone and oscilloscope ( and , suppl 4).

Figure 3. Transverse bundle shapes of the 10 channel, 2.4 MHz central frequency transducer at different depths. In these measurements, component phases are arranged as to obtain a focusing angle of 11 degrees. HIFU driver was drived at 2800 kHz with 10% amplitude. While it effects symmetrically, only the signal focused on left side is demonstrated.

Figure 3. Transverse bundle shapes of the 10 channel, 2.4 MHz central frequency transducer at different depths. In these measurements, component phases are arranged as to obtain a focusing angle of 11 degrees. HIFU driver was drived at 2800 kHz with 10% amplitude. While it effects symmetrically, only the signal focused on left side is demonstrated.

Figure 4. Alteration of the transvers bundle shape with regard to depth after rat scale is put in front of the transducer. The centre of transducer array is set to be placed in 1.8 mm front of bregma point. Thus, ultrasound wave bundles are directed on nucleus accumbens. HIFU driver's amplitude was used as 20%.

Figure 4. Alteration of the transvers bundle shape with regard to depth after rat scale is put in front of the transducer. The centre of transducer array is set to be placed in 1.8 mm front of bregma point. Thus, ultrasound wave bundles are directed on nucleus accumbens. HIFU driver's amplitude was used as 20%.

In order to place the ultrasound probe, magnets were attached to all rats’ calvaria. One day after this step, the stage was started for installing a setting that will enable the transducer to be attached on the scalp of animals. Firstly, rats were anaesthetized with xylazine (10 mg/kg) and ketamine (80 mg/kg) i.p., and then the skin of the scalp was removed. Transducer was placed on the middle line, 1.70 mm away from Bregma on the nasal side, projection of installed magnets was marked on the scalp and magnets were attached to these areas. Two circular magnets with 4 mm diameter and 2 mm thickness were used for keeping the transducer fixed on rats’ head throughout sonication period (SP) and two neodymium magnets with the same size were installed on transducer (suppl 5). Transducer was removed from the head at the end of each sonication. This process was also applied to rats in sham group, although they did not receive sonication (sham procedure). After the surgical procedure, the animals were left to healing for one day on day 15.

Sonication

On day sixteen, 18 rats in the study group were randomly divided into two, as US group to receive ultrasound stimulation and sham group to receive fake ultrasound stimulation. Both groups received normal saline injections in black compartment and morphine hydrochloride injections in white compartment for 30 min via i.p. route in 6-hour intervals twice a day for 10 days. In the meantime, 8 rats which were in physiologic saline group received only normal saline injections via i.p. route in 6-h intervals twice a day, and they were placed in black and white compartments for 30 min as in conditioning stage ().

Table 3. Conditioned place preference study progress/ sonication period.

Before morphine administrations, rats in US group and sham group received isoflurane inhaler anaesthetic (1.5%) once a day for 10 days and taken into and fastened in the brain stimulation setting (suppl 6,7). Afterwards, ultrasound gel was administered to US group and LIFU stimulation was given for 10 min. Fake ultrasound stimulation was performed on sham group. Ten minutes after the stimulation, morphine was administered to rats and they were placed in white compartment for 30 min.

Reassessment of addiction level

Post-conditioning period started one day after last injections. Sliding guillotine doors were raised and free access to all apparatus was given to rats for 900 s. Passing time of rats through settings were recorded with cameras. Assessment of CPP was calculated with the time spent in white compartment matched with morphine. On the last day of the experiment, morphine-related CPP durations of US and sham groups were assessed.

Statistical analyses

In the statistical assessment of study data, non-parametric Kruskal–Wallis test was used for comparison of more than two groups, and Dunn tests were used for subgroup analyses. For comparison of pairs, different groups were compared with non-parametric Mann–Whitney U test, and comparison of intra-group changes was performed by non-parametric Wilcoxon test. The value of .05 was assumed as the level of statistical significance in all tests.

Histologic study

Brain tissues of experiment groups were fixed for 72 h, in 10% neutral-buffered formalin (NBF). Then, they have been dehydrated through increasing series of alcohol (70%, 90%, 96% and 100%) and pellucided with xylene. Following this, they have been kept in 60°C paraffin overnight and embedded in paraffin. These paraffin blocks were cut in 5-μm thickness and placed on microscope slide. For microscopic study, they have been stained with hematoxylin–eosin. Stained sections have been observed with the light microscope (Nikon, Eclipse i5, Japan).

Immunostaining

Sections have been kept in the incubator overnight at 37°C, then, placed in, firstly pure, subsequently 96% alcohol for 10 min. Endogenous enzyme blockage was performed through being kept in 3% hydrogen peroxide prepared with methanol. After being washed firstly with tap water, then with distillated water, they were put in citrate buffer and subjected to 200 W microwave for 10 min. Sections were cooled in room temperature, then kept in blocking solution for 10 min. They were kept overnight in rabbit policlonal anti-JNK1, anti-caspase 3 and anti-MAP2 primary antibodies at +4°C. Then they were washed three times, each for 5 min with PBS, followed by marking with Alexa Fluor 488 conjugated secondary antibodies. Afterwards, they were closed, using closing matter which contains DAPI (Fluoroshield with DAPI) and viewed on confocal microscope.

A summary of the process throughout the study is presented as a flowchart on .

Table 4. Flowchart of methodology throughout the study.

Results

After the CPP protocol, three animals from US group and one animal from physiologic saline group died during anaesthesia.

For CPP, time spent in seconds (s) by rats in preliminary test stage within the white compartment and the black compartment is presented in , and the time spent by physiologic saline and morphine groups within the white compartment during preliminary test and place preference test is presented in . In , LIFU was shown to affect the place preference of rats which have developed CPP to morphine and are continuing to morphine conditioning.

Figure 5. Time spent by rats in preliminary test stage within white compartment and black compartment.

Figure 5. Time spent by rats in preliminary test stage within white compartment and black compartment.

Figure 6. Time spent in white compartment by physiologic saline and morphine groups during preliminary test and place preference test.

Figure 6. Time spent in white compartment by physiologic saline and morphine groups during preliminary test and place preference test.

Figure 7. LIFU effect on the place preference of rats.

Figure 7. LIFU effect on the place preference of rats.

Results of statistical analyses

Upon comparing the time in seconds spent by all rats in the white and black compartments in preliminary test stage, time spent in the black compartment was significantly higher than time spent in the white compartment (). Thus, the experiment procedure was prepared to suit biased design.

In preliminary test stage, no significant difference was determined upon comparing time spent in the white compartment by rats in saline [33,4 (3,4 −114,4) s] and morphine [37,2 (0–188,8) s] groups (p > .05). In place preference test stage, upon comparing time spent by groups in the white compartment, time spent by morphine group [159,7 (0,0–754,9) s] was significantly higher than the time spent by saline group [43,7 (0,3–176,3) s] (p < .05). Furthermore, time spent by morphine group within the white compartment in place preference test [159,7 (0,0–754,9) s] was significantly higher than the time spent within the white compartment in preliminary test [37,2 (0–188,8) s] (p < .01). Upon comparing the place preference test time and the preliminary test time of both groups with regard to time spent in the white compartment, time difference of morphine group [93,7 (−86,0–733,1) s] was significantly higher than time difference of saline group [1,9 ± (−45,2–148,0) s] (p < .01) ( and ).

Table 5. Comparison of saline and morphine groups with regard to time spent in white compartment and time difference.

Table 6. Comparison of saline and morphine groups with regard to time spent in white compartment in preliminary and place preference tests.

Statistical analysis has shown a serious increase in the white compartment preference of rats in sham group who are continuing morphine administration. Expected decrease could not be shown for the white compartment preference of rats in US group, however, no significant increase was observed in the white compartment preference either.

US group [165,8 (35,5–754,9) s] spent significantly more time in the white compartment compared to the saline group [43,7 (0,0–176,3) s] in place preference test stage (p < .05). There was no significant difference between time spent by sham [158,6 (0,0–687,4) s] and US [165,8 (35,5–754,9) s] groups within the white compartment in place preference test (p > .05).

In post-stimulation measurements, sham group [719,0 (41,1–828,6) s] spent significantly more time in the white compartment compared to saline group [96,3 (0,0–348,7) s] (p < .05) and no significant difference was determined between US group [450,6 (12,0–839,5) s] and saline group [96,3 (0,0–348,7) s] and between US group [450,6 (12,0–839,5) s] and sham group [719,0 (41,1–828,6) s] with regard to time spent in the white compartment.

Time spent by saline group in the white compartment after stimulation showed no significant difference compared to place preference test (pre-stimulation) stage. Time spent by sham group in the white compartment was significantly different from place preference test stage (p < .05). In addition, time spent by US group in the white compartment showed no significant difference from place preference test stage. With regard to time spent in the white compartment, upon comparing the differences of post-stimulation time and place preference test time; no significant difference was observed between time difference of saline group and time difference of sham group, despite the p value of .064. Similarly, no significant difference was observed between time difference of saline group and time difference of US group and between time difference of US group and time difference of sham group.

Results of histological examination

In all groups, histopathologic screening with hematoxylin–eosin showed the area where NA was located (). NA areas were examined in terms of impairment of tissue integration, necrosis, apoptosis, infiltration of cells and loss of myelinisation. In saline group, NA area was seen to have normal morphology. Likewise, sham and US groups were seen to have normal morphology and protected tissue integrity. US group did not demonstrate tissue damage like cell infiltration, impairment of tissue integration, necrosis or loss of myelinisation. In light microscopic study, US group and saline group showed no difference (). Immunostaining study with anti-MAP2 did not show difference between groups (). This reveals that US has no increasing or reducing effect on microtubules, which are a component of the cytoskeleton of neurons or glial cells in rats’ brain tissue sections of NA region.

Figure 8. Coronal section of nucleus accumbens of rats in saline (a), sham (b) and US (C) groups respectively. Hematoxylin–Eosin, ×1 lens.

Figure 8. Coronal section of nucleus accumbens of rats in saline (a), sham (b) and US (C) groups respectively. Hematoxylin–Eosin, ×1 lens.

Figure 9. Sections of brain tissues of Saline (A), Sham (B) and US (C) groups respectively. Nucleus accumbens around anterior commissure are seen. Hematoxylin–Eosine, ×20 lens.

Figure 9. Sections of brain tissues of Saline (A), Sham (B) and US (C) groups respectively. Nucleus accumbens around anterior commissure are seen. Hematoxylin–Eosine, ×20 lens.

Figure 10. Sections of brain tissues of Saline (A), Sham (B) and US (C) groups respectively. Anti-MAP2 immunostaining is performed to nucleus accumbens region. Microtubules stained with anti-MAP2 are seen in green colour. Nuclei are stained with DAPI (blue). Concerning anti-MAP2 immuostaining, there is not any variation between groups in terms of green fluorescent luminescence intensity (Correspondingly, US stimulation is concluded to have no harmful effect on cytoskeleton). Anti-Map2 immünostaining, DAPI, ×40 oil-immersion lens.

Figure 10. Sections of brain tissues of Saline (A), Sham (B) and US (C) groups respectively. Anti-MAP2 immunostaining is performed to nucleus accumbens region. Microtubules stained with anti-MAP2 are seen in green colour. Nuclei are stained with DAPI (blue). Concerning anti-MAP2 immuostaining, there is not any variation between groups in terms of green fluorescent luminescence intensity (Correspondingly, US stimulation is concluded to have no harmful effect on cytoskeleton). Anti-Map2 immünostaining, DAPI, ×40 oil-immersion lens.

After anti-JNK1 immunostaining, no positive cells were found in blood vessels or neuronal tissue in NA region ( and ). While these antibodies are indicators of apoptosis, this finding exhibits that there were no apoptotic cells in NA region of any group. Thus, US stimulation does not seem to have apoptotic effect on NA area.

Figure 11. Anti-JNK1 immünostaining. For positive control of Anti-JNK1 antibody, blood vessel is stained. Nuclei stained with DAPI (blue), anti-JNK1 positive cells on blood vessel (green) and internal elastic membrane located in intima layer of blood vessel (red autofluorescent luminescence) are seen, x40 oil-immersion lens.

Figure 11. Anti-JNK1 immünostaining. For positive control of Anti-JNK1 antibody, blood vessel is stained. Nuclei stained with DAPI (blue), anti-JNK1 positive cells on blood vessel (green) and internal elastic membrane located in intima layer of blood vessel (red autofluorescent luminescence) are seen, x40 oil-immersion lens.

Figure 12. Anti-JNK1 immünostaining. Saline (A), Sham (B) and US (C) groups respectively are stained with anti- JNK1 antibody. None of the sections of NA region within all groups included positive cells. Nuclei are stained with DAPI (blue), ×40 oil-immersion lens.

Figure 12. Anti-JNK1 immünostaining. Saline (A), Sham (B) and US (C) groups respectively are stained with anti- JNK1 antibody. None of the sections of NA region within all groups included positive cells. Nuclei are stained with DAPI (blue), ×40 oil-immersion lens.

Brain sections were stained with Fluoro- Jade C, which is used to determine the neuronal damage. There were no damaged neurons found in NA area, among any group. Thus, US stimulation does not seem to have any neuronal damage effect.

Discussion

The purpose of the current study was to observe morphine-conditioned place preference in rats by stimulating NA with a non-invasive method, focused ultrasound and to detect whether there would be any tissue damage caused by US waves. No significant tissue damage caused by LIFU waves was detected. Place preference scores of sham group increased significantly, while scores of LIFU group did not and this result may be associated with LIFU stimulation. Although LIFU stimulation seemed to stop the increase of morphine-induced place preference scores, it was not found to cause a significant decrease in those scores.

US performs a mechanical pressure when passing through one side of the tissue to the other side [Citation23]. The underlying source of ultrasonic neuromodulation is still not clear, despite being the subject of the original study of Harvey [Citation24]. Biological effects of US are primarily thermal at high intensity [Citation25]. For US wave form, possible cellular mechanisms have been defined as cavitation, thermogenic effects and mechanic stimulation [Citation26]. Unlike HIFU, effects of LIFU waves are not thought to be thermogenic. Investigators have suggested that neuromodulation provided with short pulses in LIFU may alter the neural conduction and may cause potential changes via mechanic path rather than thermal path [Citation14]. There are studies showing that focused ultrasound waves can change action potentials in hippocampal sections of rats [Citation27], can quench stimulated potentials of rat hippocampus [Citation7], and that stimulation and subsequent reduction in action potential is obtained with short (0.5 milliseconds) focused ultrasound blasts in sciatic nerve preparation of frogs [Citation28]. There are hypotheses stating that, US induced neuromodulation occurs through mechanical stress of lipid layer [Citation29].

No changes were observed in the structure of neural membranes even with chronic stimulation of LIFU waves [Citation9]. Low frequency and low energy LIFU waves are much lower than the damage-causing threshold value. Tissue may not be damaged at all even with high frequency and high energy [Citation30,Citation31]. The possible increase in temperature with LIFU is not in a level to pose negative effects on the neuronal activity [Citation32]. For this reason, LIFU waves may alter the neuronal activity probably without causing tissue injuries.

In a study, the activity of the primary somatosensory cortices of humans was shown to be modulated with LIFU waves [Citation15]. Focused ultrasound applied on thalamus has been shown to reduce the time of voluntary movement and reflex response to pinching in anaesthetized rats [Citation33]. Focused ultrasound may also help the growth of new nerves. In a study, micro bubble-enhanced focused ultrasound was shown to increase hippocampal neurogenesis in the mice [Citation34]. Therefore, LIFU waves may be suggested for use in neurodegenerative disorders, particularly Alzheimer's. There are studies showing that focused ultrasound may even affect nerve cell growth and morphology [Citation35].

Morphine-conditioned place preference scores were shown in the first stage of the current study, in line with the findings of DBS studies. However, on the second stage, positive effects of LIFU waves on morphine dependency could not be shown significantly as performed in DBS studies and morphine-conditioned place preference scores have not decreased. On the other hand, it can be stated that there is no significant increase in morphine-seeking behaviour of US group rats which have developed morphine-conditioned place preference and are continuing morphine conditioning. Meanwhile, morphine-seeking behaviour was seriously increased in the sham group which continued receiving morphine. While there is a significant difference in morphine-seeking behaviour upon comparison of US group with saline group, this difference became insignificant after ultrasound stimulation.

The reason for inability to show the positive effects of LIFU waves on morphine dependency as in DBS studies may be that LIFU stimulation duration was relatively short. Besides, in the study, stimulation was started immediately after conditioning procedure, however, dependency learning behaviour may have been developed in conditioning stage. Although morphine-seeking behaviour may be attenuated by LIFU, stimulation for such a short time may have failed in changing the already learned behaviour since then.

In our study, NA focused LIFU waves were estimated to form a stimulant effect through mechanosensitive channels by activating voltage-gated ion channels mechanically and with stress. By this way, LIFU stimulation was thought to effect as a partial agonist and cause a partial dopaminergic increase in NA, and therefore it was considered to prevent the increase in morphine-seeking behaviour of rats. However we did not aim to show the mechanism affecting the present neurotransmission, which might be a subject of further studies.

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study that examines LIFU stimulation effects on dependency behaviour. Moreover, while animals have received ultrasound stimulation under anaesthesia in all previous studies, only inhaler isoflurane was used in this study and rats were taken in custom-made stabilizing devices we have ordered. Stereotactic magnet-attachment procedure according to the point of ultrasound probe that takes Bregma point as reference was also performed firstly in this study.

One animal from physiologic saline group and 3 animals from US group died during anaesthesia procedures. These deaths have lowered the number of rats in both groups (7 animals in physiologic saline group, 6 animals in US group) which may have diminished statistical power. Increasing the number of animals in further studies performed with a similar methodology may contribute to the significance of results.

Prolonged anaesthesia is a potential risk for death of at least a part of experimental animals. Thus, future studies should consider selecting anaesthetics with low mortality rates.

Current study showed that dependency behaviour of sham group is increased significantly, but no significant increase was shown in US group. In the future, more evident results may be obtained by planning treatment modalities with longer stimulation. In this case, adjusting morphine doses may be beneficial since morphine tolerance may be developed with the longer conditioning period that is required, or the effect of LIFU on morphine dependence may be studied on different addiction methods. An experimental model which excludes the effect of conditioned learning, like self-administration, may be helpful to detect the exact role of LIFU on morphine dependence. However, this may require a remarkably high study funding.

Conclusions

The purpose of this study was to observe the change in morphine-dependent place preference by the stimulation of NA with a non-invasive method, focused ultrasound, in a CPP model with rats which are morphine-conditioned and continuing morphine-conditioning.

Although morphine-dependent place preference scores of US group were not decreased in the study, no significant increase was shown in morphine-seeking behaviour compared to pre-stimulation period, and this result may be associated with LIFU stimulation.

The limitations of this study include limited funding resulting in the inability to show with microdialysis method whether the present neurotransmission is also a significant change in neuronal activation or not, and the fact that rat deaths lowered the number of animals required for strong statistical significance. Considering the number of deaths, increasing the number of animals in further studies performed with a similar methodology may contribute to the significance of results. Another remarkable limitation is that we used i.p. route for morphine injections. While morphine has a marked hepatic first-pass metabolism, this application route may have altered the exact effects of morphine. Further, we possibly sonicated the whole NA, both shell and core parts, which may well effect the interpretations of outcomes.

We think that the innovation of this study will provide new approaches for LIFU studies in both establishing longer treatment modalities and its use for treatment purposes. In the future, more significant results may be obtained with different dependency methods and by planning treatment modalities with longer stimulation.

We consider that novel studies are required on LIFU stimulation as an alternative method in the treatment of morphine and other opioid dependencies, which have become a serious medical and social issue.

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.

Additional information

Funding

This work was supported by the Bezmiâlem Vakif Üniversitesi [grant number 9.2015/20]; Türkiye Bilimsel ve Teknolojik Araştirma Kurumu [grant number 214S249].

References

  • Compton P, Darakjian J, Miotto K. Screening for addiction in patients with chronic pain and “problematic” substance use: evaluation of a pilot assessment tool. J Pain Symptom Manage. 1998;16(6):355–363.
  • Koob GF, Volkow ND. Neurocircuitry of addiction. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35(1):217–238.
  • Fecteau S, Fregni F, Boggio PS, et al. Neuromodulation of decision-making in the addictive brain. Subst Use Misuse. 2010;45(11):1766–1786.
  • Kuhn J, Lenartz D, Huff W, et al. Remission of alcohol dependency following deep brain stimulation of the nucleus accumbens: valuable therapeutic implications? J Neurol, Neurosurg & Psychiatry. 2007;78(10):1152–1153.
  • Liu HY, Jin J, Tang JS, et al. Preclinical study: chronic deep brain stimulation in the rat nucleus accumbens and its effect on morphine reinforcement. Addict Biol. 2008;13(1):40–46.
  • Wang L, Wang G, Zhao Y. Effect of deep brain stimulation of nucleus accumbens on psychological morphine dependence in rats. Chin J Minimally Invasive Neurosurg. 2008;5:016.
  • Mihran RT, Barnes FS, Wachtel H. Temporally-specific modification of myelinated axon excitability in vitro following a single ultrasound pulse. Ultrasound Med Biol. 1990;16(3):297–309.
  • Baizabal Carvallo JF, Simpson R, Jankovic J. Diagnosis and treatment of complications related to deep brain stimulation hardware. Mov Disord. 2011;26(8):1398–1406.
  • Tyler WJ, Tufail Y, Finsterwald M, et al. Remote excitation of neuronal circuits using low-intensity, low-frequency ultrasound. Public Libr Sci. 2008;3(10):e3511.
  • Tyler WJ. Noninvasive neuromodulation with ultrasound? A continuum mechanics hypothesis. Neuroscientist. 2011;17(1):25–36.
  • Bystritsky A, Korb AS, Douglas PK, et al. A review of low-intensity focused ultrasound pulsation. Brain Stimul. 2011;4(3):125–136.
  • Min BK, Bystritsky A, Jung KI, et al. Focused ultrasound-mediated suppression of chemically-induced acute epileptic EEG activity. BMC Neurosci. 2011;12(1):23.
  • Fry WJ, Fry RB. A possible mechanism involved in the conduction process of thin sheathed nerve fibers. J Cell Physiol. 1950;36(2):229–239.
  • Heckman JD, Ryaby JP, McCabe J, et al. Acceleration of tibial fracture-healing by non-invasive, low-intensity pulsed ultrasound. J Bone Joint Surg-Am Volume. 1994;76(1):26–34.
  • Legon W, Sato TF, Opitz A, et al. Transcranial focused ultrasound modulates the activity of primary somatosensory cortex in humans. Nat Neurosci. 2014;17(2):322–329.
  • Roma PG, Riley AL. Apparatus bias and the use of light and texture in place conditioning. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2005;82(1):163–169.
  • Ferbinteanu J, McDonald RJ. Dorsal/ventral hippocampus, fornix, and conditioned place preference. Hippocampus. 2001;11(2):187–200.
  • Mucha RF, Van Der Kooy D, O’Shaughnessy M, et al. Drug reinforcement studied by the use of place conditioning in rat. Brain Res. 1982;243(1):91–105.
  • Chen Q, Hou H, Feng J, et al. PET Imaging Reveals Brain Metabolic Changes in Adolescent Rats following Chronic Escalating Morphine Administration. Mol Imaging Biol. 2018;20(6):993–1000.
  • Moon S, Kang S, Shin H, et al. Morphine dependence is attenuated by treatment of 3, 4, 5-trimethoxy cinnamic acid in mice and rats. Neurochem Res. 2019;44(4):874–883.
  • Chen SL, Tao PL, Chu CH, et al. Low-dose memantine attenuated morphine addictive behavior through its anti-inflammation and neurotrophic effects in rats. J Neuroimmune Pharmacol. 2012;7(2):444–453.
  • Wang F, Jing X, Yang J, et al. The role of the insular cortex in naloxone-induced conditioned place aversion in morphine-dependent mice. Physiol Res. 2016;65(4):701.
  • Kubanek J, Shukla P, Das A, et al. Ultrasound elicits behavioral responses through mechanical effects on neurons and ion channels in a simple nervous system. J Neurosci. 2018;38(12):3081–3091.
  • Harvey EN. The effect of high frequency sound waves on heart muscle and other irritable tissues. Am J Physiol. 1929;91(1):284–290.
  • Tsui PH, Wang SH, Huang CC. In vitro effects of ultrasound with different energies on the conduction properties of neural tissue. Ultrasonics. 2005;43(7):560–565.
  • Bachtold MR, Rinaldi PC, Jones JP, et al. Focused ultrasound modifications of neural circuit activity in a mammalian brain. Ultrasound Med Biol. 1998;24(4):557–565.
  • Rinaldi PC, Jones JP, Reines F, et al. Modification by focused ultrasound pulses of electrically evoked responses from an in vitro hippocampal preparation. Brain Res. 1991;558(1):36–42.
  • Foley JL, Little JW, Vaezy S. Effects of high-intensity focused ultrasound on nerve conduction. Muscle Nerve. 2008;37(2):241–250.
  • Tufail Y, Matyushov A, Baldwin N, et al. Transcranial pulsed ultrasound stimulates intact brain circuits. Neuron. 2010;66(5):681–694.
  • Dalecki D. Mechanical bioeffects of ultrasound. Annu Rev Biomed Eng. 2004;6:229–248.
  • Dinno MA, Dyson M, Young SR, et al. The significance of membrane changes in the safe and effective use of therapeutic and diagnostic ultrasound. Phys Med Biol. 1989;34(11):1543.
  • O’Brien WD. Ultrasound-biophysics mechanisms. Prog Biophys Mol Biol. 2007;93:212–255.
  • Yoo SS, Kim H, Min BK, et al. Transcranial focused ultrasound to the thalamus alters anesthesia time in rats. NeuroReport. 2011;22(15):783–787.
  • Scarcelli T, Jordão JF, O’Reilly MA, et al. Stimulation of hippocampal neurogenesis by transcranial focused ultrasound and microbubbles in adult mice. Brain Stimul. 2014;7(2):304–307.
  • Hu Y, Zhong W, Wan JM, et al. Ultrasound can modulate neuronal development: impact on neurite growth and cell body morphology. Ultrasound Med Biol. 2013;39(5):915–925.