Publication Cover
Leisure Sciences
An Interdisciplinary Journal
Volume 46, 2024 - Issue 3
226
Views
2
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Review Articles

Setting Our Sights on Vision: A Rationale and Research Agenda for Integrating Eye-Tracking into Leisure Research

ORCID Icon
Pages 274-295 | Received 24 Sep 2020, Accepted 29 Jun 2021, Published online: 24 Jul 2021

References

  • Adolphs, R. (2008). Fear, faces, and the human amygdala. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 18(2), 166–172. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2008.06.006
  • Areni, C. S., & Kim, D. (1993). The influence of background music on shopping behavior: classical versus top-forty music in a wine store. ACR North American Advances, 20, 336–340.
  • Auyeung, B., Lombardo, M. V., Heinrichs, M., Chakrabarti, B., Sule, A., Deakin, J. B., Bethlehem, R. A. I., Dickens, L., Mooney, N., Sipple, J. A. N., Thiemann, P., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2015). Oxytocin increases eye contact during a real-time, naturalistic social interaction in males with and without autism. Translational Psychiatry, 5(2), e507. https://doi.org/10.1038/tp.2014.146
  • Awh, E., Vogel, E. K., & Oh, S. H. (2006). Interactions between attention and working memory. Neuroscience, 139(1), 201–208. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.08.023
  • Balcetis, E., & Dunning, D. (2006). See what you want to see: Motivational influences on visual perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(4), 612–625. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.91.4.612
  • Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., DeWall, C. N., & Zhang, L. (2007). How emotion shapes behavior: Feedback, anticipation, and reflection, rather than direct causation. Personality and Social Psychology Review: An Official Journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc, 11(2), 167–203. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868307301033
  • Benfield, J. A., Taff, B. D., Newman, P., & Smyth, J. (2014). Natural sound facilitates mood recovery. Ecopsychology, 6(3), 183–188.
  • Bitgood, S. (2010). An attention-value model of museum visitors. Center for Advancement of Informal Science Education.
  • Boccia, M. L., Melton, K., & Larson, M. (2020). An overview of the use of oxytocin measures in leisure studies. Journal of Leisure Research, 51(3), 366–376. https://doi.org/10.1080/00222216.2019.1687265
  • Broadbet, D. E. (1958). Perception and communication. Pergamon Press.
  • Browning, M. H., Mimnaugh, K. J., van Riper, C. J., Laurent, H. K., & LaValle, S. M. (2019). Can simulated nature support mental health? Comparing short, single-doses of 360-degree nature videos in virtual reality with the outdoors. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 2667.
  • Buschman, T. J., & Miller, E. K. (2007). Top-down versus bottom-up control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices. Science (New York, N.Y.), 315(5820), 1860–1862. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1138071
  • Corbetta, M., & Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 3(3), 201–215.
  • Cottet, M., Vaudor, L., Tronchère, H., Roux-Michollet, D., Augendre, M., & Brault, V. (2018). Using gaze behavior to gain insights into the impacts of naturalness on city dwellers' perceptions and valuation of a landscape. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 60, 9–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2018.09.001
  • Crompton, J. L. (2016). Implications of prospect theory for the pricing of leisure services. Leisure Sciences, 38(4), 315–337. https://doi.org/10.1080/01490400.2015.1107516
  • Curcio, C. A., Sloan, K. R., Kalina, R. E., & Hendrickson, A. E. (1990). Human photoreceptor topography. The Journal of Comparative Neurology, 292(4), 497–523. https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.902920402
  • Desimone, R., & Duncan, J. (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 18(1), 193–222. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ne.18.030195.001205
  • Dijksterhuis, A., & Aarts, H. (2010). Goals, attention, and (un)consciousness. Annual Review of Psychology, 61, 467–490. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100445
  • Duchowski, A. T. (2002). A breadth-first survey of eye-tracking applications. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers: A Journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc, 34(4), 455–470. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03195475
  • Duchowski, A. T. (2007). Eye tracking methodology: Theory and practice. Springer-Verlag.
  • Duncan, R. O., & Boynton, G. M. (2003). Cortical magnification within human primary visual cortex correlates with acuity thresholds. Neuron, 38(4), 659–671. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00265-4
  • Dustin, D. L., Zajchowski, C. A., & Schwab, K. A. (2019). The biochemistry behind human behavior: Implications for leisure sciences and services. Leisure Sciences, 41(6), 542–549. https://doi.org/10.1080/01490400.2019.1597793
  • Farber, M. E., & Hall, T. E. (2007). Emotion and environment: Visitors' extraordinary experiences along the Dalton Highway in Alaska. Journal of Leisure Research, 39(2), 248–270. https://doi.org/10.1080/00222216.2007.11950107
  • Furman, N., Shooter, W., & Schumann, S. (2010). The roles of heuristics, avalanche forecast, and risk propensity in the decision making of backcountry skiers. Leisure Sciences, 32(5), 453–469. https://doi.org/10.1080/01490400.2010.510967
  • Gamberini, L., Spagnolli, A., Corradi, N., Sartori, G., Ghirardi, V., & Jacucci, G. (2014). Combining implicit and explicit techniques to reveal social desirability bias in electricity conservation self-reports. Energy Efficiency, 7(6), 923–935. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12053-014-9266-6
  • Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2016). The interaction between vision and eye movements†. Perception, 45(12), 1333–1357. https://doi.org/10.1177/0301006616657097
  • Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Psychology Press.
  • Grimm, P. (2010). Social desirability bias. In Wiley international encyclopedia of marketing. Wiley.
  • Guan, Z., Lee, S., Cuddihy, E., & Ramey, J. (2006, April). The validity of the stimulated retrospective think-aloud method as measured by eye tracking. In Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (pp. 1253–1262). ACM. https://doi.org/10.1145/1124772.1124961
  • Guestrin, E. D., & Eizenman, M. (2006). General theory of remote gaze estimation using the pupil center and corneal reflections. IEEE Transactions on Bio-Medical Engineering, 53(6), 1124–1133. https://doi.org/10.1109/TBME.2005.863952
  • Haxby, J. V., Hoffman, E. A., & Gobbini, M. I. (2002). Human neural systems for face recognition and social communication. Biological Psychiatry, 51(1), 59–67. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01330-0
  • Hicks, J. (2018). Exploring the relationship between awe and leisure: A conceptual argument. Journal of Leisure Research, 49(3–5), 258–276. https://doi.org/10.1080/00222216.2018.1535868
  • Isaacowitz, D. M., Wadlinger, H. A., Goren, D., & Wilson, H. R. (2006). Selective preference in visual fixation away from negative images in old age? An eye-tracking study. Psychology and Aging, 21(1), 40–48. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.21.1.40
  • Iso-Ahola, S. E. (2015). Conscious versus nonconscious mind and leisure. Leisure Sciences, 37(4), 289–310. https://doi.org/10.1080/01490400.2015.1005560
  • Kandel, E. R., Schwartz, J. H., & Jessell, T. M. (2012). Principles of neural science. McGraw-Hill.
  • Kastner, S., & Ungerleider, L. G. (2000). Mechanisms of visual attention in the human cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 23, 315–341. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.315
  • Kiefer, P., Giannopoulos, I., Kremer, D., Schlieder, C., & Raubal, M. (2014). Starting to get bored: An outdoor eye tracking study of tourists exploring a city panorama. In Proceedings of the Symposium on Eye Tracking Research and Applications (pp. 315–318). ACM.
  • Kohlhardt, R., Honey-Rosés, J., Fernandez Lozada, S., Haider, W., & Stevens, M. (2018). Is this trail too crowded? A choice experiment to evaluate tradeoffs and preferences of park visitors in Garibaldi Park, British Columbia. Journal of Environmental Planning and Management, 61(1), 1–24. https://doi.org/10.1080/09640568.2017.1284047
  • Kollmuss, A., & Agyeman, J. (2002). Mind the gap: Why do people act environmentally and what are the barriers to pro-environmental behavior? Environmental Education Research, 8(3), 239–260. https://doi.org/10.1080/13504620220145401
  • Kormos, C., & Gifford, R. (2014). The validity of self-report measures of proenvironmental behavior: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 40, 359–371. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2014.09.003
  • Larson, L. R., Usher, L. E., & Chapmon, T. (2018). Surfers as environmental stewards: Understanding place-protecting behavior at Cape Hatteras National Seashore. Leisure Sciences, 40(5), 442–465. https://doi.org/10.1080/01490400.2017.1305306
  • Lee, C., Payne, L. L., & Berdychevsky, L. (2020). The roles of leisure attitudes and self-efficacy on attitudes toward retirement among retirees: A sense of coherence theory approach. Leisure Sciences, 42(2), 152–169.
  • Lee, B., & Shafer, C. S. (2002). The dynamic nature of leisure experience: An application of affect control theory. Journal of Leisure Research, 34(3), 290–310. https://doi.org/10.1080/00222216.2002.11949973
  • Lewin, K. (1951). Field theory in social science. Selected theoretical papers (D. Cartwright, Ed.). Harpers.
  • Liberman, V., Samuels, S. M., & Ross, L. (2004). The name of the game: Predictive power of reputations versus situational labels in determining prisoner’s dilemma game moves. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30(9), 1175–1185. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167204264004
  • Liu, D., & Upchurch, R. S. (2020). A glimpse into energy tourism via application of eye-tracking technology. Journal of Leisure Research, 51(2), 230–244. https://doi.org/10.1080/00222216.2019.1649098
  • Liversedge, S. P., & Findlay, J. M. (2000). Saccadic eye movements and cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(1), 6–14. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(99)01418-7
  • Mack, A. (2003). Inattentional blindness: Looking without seeing. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12(5), 180–184. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.01256
  • Manning, R., Lawson, S., Newman, P., Laven, D., & Valliere, W. (2002). Methodological issues in measuring crowding-related norms in outdoor recreation. Leisure Sciences, 24(3–4), 339–348. https://doi.org/10.1080/01490400290050781
  • Marois, R., & Ivanoff, J. (2005). Capacity limits of information processing in the brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(6), 296–305. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.04.010
  • Maughan, L., Gutnikov, S., & Stevens, R. (2007). Like more, look more. Look more, like more: The evidence from eye-tracking. Journal of Brand Management, 14(4), 335–342. https://doi.org/10.1057/palgrave.bm.2550074
  • Meißner, M., Pfeiffer, J., Pfeiffer, T., & Oppewal, H. (2019). Combining virtual reality and mobile eye tracking to provide a naturalistic experimental environment for shopper research. Journal of Business Research, 100, 445–458. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2017.09.028
  • Miller, Z. D., Freimund, W., Metcalf, E. C., & Nickerson, N. (2018). Targeting your audience: Wildlife value orientations and the relevance of messages about bear safety. Human Dimensions of Wildlife, 23(3), 213–226. https://doi.org/10.1080/10871209.2017.1409371
  • Morris, E., Van Riper, C. J., Kyle, G. T., Wallen, K. E., & Absher, J. (2018). Accounting for gender in a study of the motivation-involvement relationship. Leisure Sciences, 40(6), 494–507. https://doi.org/10.1080/01490400.2016.1256799
  • Ngan, H. F. B., Bavik, A., Kuo, C. F., & Yu, C. E. (2020). Where you look depends on what you are willing to afford: Eye tracking in menus. Journal of Hospitality & Tourism Research, 1096348020951226.
  • Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84(3), 231–259. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.84.3.231
  • Nyström, M., & Holmqvist, K. (2010). An adaptive algorithm for fixation, saccade, and glissade detection in eyetracking data. Behavior Research Methods, 42(1), 188–204. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.42.1.188
  • Öhman, A., Flykt, A., & Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: Detecting the snake in the grass. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130(3), 466–478. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.130.3.466
  • Parkhurst, D., Law, K., & Niebur, E. (2002). Modeling the role of salience in the allocation of overt visual attention. Vision Research, 42(1), 107–123. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0042-6989(01)00250-4
  • Pessoa, L. (2005). To what extent are emotional visual stimuli processed without attention and awareness? Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 15(2), 188–196. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2005.03.002
  • Posner, M. I. (1994). Attention: The mechanisms of consciousness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 91(16), 7398–7403. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.91.16.7398
  • Rainoldi, M., & Jooss, M. (Eds.). (2020). Eye tracking in tourism. Springer.
  • Ross, J., Morrone, M. C., Goldberg, M. E., & Burr, D. C. (2001). Changes in visual perception at the time of saccades. Trends in Neurosciences, 24(2), 113–121. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0166-2236(00)01685-4
  • Salvucci, D. D., & Goldberg, J. H. (2000). Identifying fixations and saccades in eye-tracking protocols. In Proceedings of the 2000 Symposium on Eye Tracking Research & Applications (pp. 71–78). ACM.
  • Schiessl, M., Duda, S., Thölke, A., & Fischer, R. (2003). Eye tracking and its application in usability and media research. MMI-Interaktiv Journal, 6, 41–50.
  • Scott, N., Green, C., & Fairley, S. (2016). Investigation of the use of eye tracking to examine tourism advertising effectiveness. Current Issues in Tourism, 19(7), 634–642. https://doi.org/10.1080/13683500.2014.1003797
  • Scott, N., Le, D., Becken, S., & Connolly, R. M. (2020). Measuring perceived beauty of the Great Barrier Reef using eye-tracking technology. Current Issues in Tourism, 23(20), 2492–2502. https://doi.org/10.1080/13683500.2019.1626812
  • Scott, N., Zhang, R., Le, D., & Moyle, B. (2019). A review of eye-tracking research in tourism. Current Issues in Tourism, 22(10), 1244–1261. https://doi.org/10.1080/13683500.2017.1367367
  • Shen, Y. S., Lever, M., & Joppe, M. (2020). Investigating the appeal of a visitor guide: a triangulated approach. International Journal of Contemporary Hospitality Management, 32(4), 1539–1562. https://doi.org/10.1108/IJCHM-03-2019-0281
  • Simons, D. J., & Chabris, C. F. (1999). Gorillas in our midst: Sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception, 28(9), 1059–1074. https://doi.org/10.1068/p281059
  • Skibins, J. C., Powell, R. B., & Stern, M. J. (2012). Exploring empirical support for interpretation's best practices. Journal of Interpretation Research, 17(1), 25–44. https://doi.org/10.1177/109258721201700103
  • Skibins, J. C., & Sharp, R. L. (2017). Evaluation of the brown bear viewing experience at Katmai National Park and Preserve: Implications for management. Human Dimensions of Wildlife, 22(5), 476–482. https://doi.org/10.1080/10871209.2017.1336584
  • Sollberger, S., Bernauer, T., & Ehlert, U. (2017). Predictors of visual attention to climate change images: An eye-tracking study. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 51, 46–56. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2017.03.001
  • Spiers, H. J., & Maguire, E. A. (2008). The dynamic nature of cognition during wayfinding. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 28(3), 232–249. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2008.02.006
  • Steckenreuter, A., & Wolf, I. D. (2013). How to use persuasive communication to encourage visitors to pay park user fees. Tourism Management, 37, 58–70. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tourman.2013.01.010
  • Stewart, W. P., & Floyd, M. F. (2004). Visualizing leisure. Journal of Leisure Research, 36(4), 445–460. https://doi.org/10.1080/00222216.2004.11950031
  • Taplin, R. H., Rodger, K., & Moore, S. A. (2016). A method for testing the effect of management interventions on the satisfaction and loyalty of national park visitors. Leisure Sciences, 38(2), 140–160. https://doi.org/10.1080/01490400.2015.1077178
  • Tappe, K. A., & Glanz, K. (2013). Measurement of exercise habits and prediction of leisure-time activity in established exercise. Psychology, Health & Medicine, 18(5), 601–611. https://doi.org/10.1080/13548506.2013.764458
  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science (New York, N.Y.), 185(4157), 1124–1131. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.185.4157.1124
  • Valtchanov, D., & Ellard, C. G. (2015). Cognitive and affective responses to natural scenes: effects of low level visual properties on preference, cognitive load and eye-movements. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 43, 184–195. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2015.07.001
  • van Riper, C. J., Landon, A. C., Kidd, S., Bitterman, P., Fitzgerald, L. A., Granek, E. F., Ibarra, S., Iwaniec, D., Raymond, C. M., & Toledo, D. (2017). Incorporating sociocultural phenomena into ecosystem-service valuation: The importance of critical pluralism. BioScience, 67(3), 233–244. https://doi.org/10.1093/biosci/biw170
  • Vuilleumier, P. (2005). How brains beware: Neural mechanisms of emotional attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(12), 585–594. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.10.011
  • Whalen, P. J., Rauch, S. L., Etcoff, N. L., McInerney, S. C., Lee, M. B., & Jenike, M. A. (1998). Masked presentations of emotional facial expressions modulate amygdala activity without explicit knowledge. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 18(1), 411–418. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-01-00411.1998
  • Williams, D. R., Patterson, M. E., Roggenbuck, J. W., & Watson, A. E. (1992). Beyond the commodity metaphor: Examining emotional and symbolic attachment to place. Leisure Sciences, 14(1), 29–46. https://doi.org/10.1080/01490409209513155
  • Wolfe, J. M., Kluender, K. R., Levi, D. M., Bartoshuk, L. M., Herz, R. S., Klatzky, R. L., Lederman, S. J., & Merfeld, D. M. (2006). Sensation & perception. Sinauer.
  • Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 35(2), 151–175. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.35.2.151

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.