86
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Is “memory-for-when” universal? Group and individual variability in temporal position memory for words, faces, and classroomsOpen Data

, , &
Pages 320-338 | Received 26 Sep 2023, Accepted 01 Feb 2024, Published online: 18 Feb 2024

References

  • Adams, R. L., & Delaney, P. F. (2023). Do we remember when to better recall what? Repetition benefits are probably not due to explicit temporal context memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 131, 104415. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.01502006.x
  • Alvarez, G. A., & Cavanagh, P. (2004). The capacity of visual short-term memory is set both by visual information load and by number of objects. Psychological Science, 15(2), 106–111. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.01502006.x
  • Anderson, J. R., & Bower, G. H. (1972). Recognition and retrieval processes in free recall. Psychological Review, 79(2), 97–123. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0033773
  • Aslan, A., Zellner, M., & Bauml, K.-H. (2010). Working memory capacity predicts listwise directed forgetting in adults and children. Memory (Hove, England), 18(4), 442–450. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211003742698
  • Auday, B. C., Sullivan, C., & Cross, H. A. (1988). The effect of constrained rehearsal on judgments of temporal order. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 26(6), 548–551. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03330118
  • Aue, W. R., Criss, A. H., & Fischetti, N. W. (2012). Associative information in memory: Evidence from cued recall. Journal of Memory and Language, 66(1), 109–122. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2011.08.002
  • Awh, E., Barton, B., & Vogel, E. K. (2007). Visual working memory represents a fixed number of items regardless of complexity. Psychological Science, 18(7), 622–628. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01949.x
  • Bainbridge, W. A., Isola, P., & Oliva, A. (2013). The intrinsic memorability of face photographs. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(4), 1323–1334. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033872
  • Batchelder, W. H., & Riefer, D. M. (1990). Multinomial processing models of source monitoring. Psychological Review, 97(4), 548–564. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.97.4.548
  • Bayen, U. J., Murnane, K., & Erdfelder, E. (1996). Source discrimination, item detection, and multinomial models of source monitoring. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22(1), 197–215. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.22.1.197
  • Brelsford, J., Freund, R., & Rundus, D. (1967). Recency judgments in a short-term memory task. Psychonomic Science, 8(6), 247–248. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03331644
  • Brown, G. D. A., & Chater, N. (2001). The chronological organization of memory: Common psychological foundations for remembering and timing. In C. Hoerl & T. McCormack (Eds.), Time and memory: Issues in philosophy and psychology (pp. 77–110). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780198250357.003.0004
  • Brown, G. D. A., Neath, I., & Chater, N. (2007). A temporal ratio model of memory. Psychological Review, 114(3), 539–576. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.114.3.539
  • Caplan, J. B. (2005). Associative isolation: Unifying associative and list memory. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 49(5), 383–402. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jmp.2005.06.004
  • Carpenter, S. K., Pashler, H., & Vul, E. (2006). What types of learning are enhanced by a cued recall test? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13(5), 826–830. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03194004
  • Coltheart, M. (1981). The MRC psycholinguistic database. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology, 33A(4), 497–505. https://doi.org/10.1080/14640748108400805
  • Cortis, C., Dent, K., Kennett, S., & Ward, G. (2015). First things first: Similar list length and output order effects for verbal and nonverbal stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41(4), 1179–1214. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000086
  • Criss, A. H., & Shiffrin, R. M. (2004). Pairs do not suffer interference from other types of pairs or single items in associative recognition. Memory & Cognition, 32(8), 1284–1297. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03206319
  • Delaney, P. F., & Sahakyan, L. (2007). Unexpected costs of high working memory capacity following directed forgetting and contextual change manipulations. Memory & Cognition, 35(5), 1074 –1082. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193479
  • Delaney, P. F., & Verkoeijen, P. P. J. L. (2009). Rehearsal strategies can enlarge or diminish the spacing effect: Pure versus mixed lists and encoding strategy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35(5), 1148–1161. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016380
  • DuBrow, S., & Davachi, L. (2014). Temporal memory is shaped by encoding stability and intervening item reactivation. The Journal of Neuroscience, 34(42), 13998–14005. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2535-14.2014
  • Eimer, M. (2018). What do associations and dissociations between face and object recognition abilities tell US about the domain-generality of face processing? Cognitive Neuropsychology, 35(1-2), 80–82. https://doi.org/10.1080/02643294.2017.1414691
  • Ensor, T. M., Guitard, D., Bireta, T. J., Hockley, W. E., & Surprenant, A. M. (2020). The list-length effect occurs in cued recall with the retroactive design but not the proactive design. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology / Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Expérimentale, 74(1), 12–24. https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000187
  • Epstein, R., & Kanwisher, N. (1998). A cortical representation of the local visual environment. Nature, 392(6676), 598–601. https://doi.org/10.1038/33402
  • Estes, W. K. (1955). Statistical theory of spontaneous recovery and regression. Psychological Review, 62(3), 145–154. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0048509
  • Flexser, A. J., & Bower, G. H. (1974). How frequency affects recency judgments: A model for recency discrimination. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 103(4), 706–716. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0037194
  • Fozard, J. L. (1970). Apparent recency of unrelated pictures and nouns presented in the same sequence. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 86(2), 137–143. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0029983
  • Gallistel, C. (1990). The organization of learning. MIT Press.
  • Gerlach, C., & Starrfelt, R. (2018). Global precedence effects account for individual differences in both face and object recognition performance. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(4), 1365–1372. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-018-1458-1
  • Geskin, J., & Behrmann, M. (2018). Congenital prosopagnosia without object agnosia? A literature review. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 35(1-2), 4–54. https://doi.org/10.1080/02643294.2017.1392295
  • Golomb, J. D., Peelle, J. E., Addis, K. M., Kahana, M. J., & Wingfield, A. (2008). Effects of adult aging on utilization of temporal and semantic associations during free and serial recall. Memory & Cognition, 36(5), 947–956. https://doi.org/10.3758/MC.36.5.947
  • Grove, K. L., & Wilding, E. L. (2009). Retrieval processes supporting judgments of recency. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(3), 461–473. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21040
  • Guida, A., Carnet, S., Normandon, M., & Lavielle-Guida, M. (2018). Can spatialisation be extended to episodic memory and open sets? Memory (Hove, England), 26(7), 922–935. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2018.1428350
  • Guida, A., & Lavielle-Guida, M. (2014). 2011 space odyssey: Spatialization as a mechanism to code order allows a close encounter between memory expertise and classic immediate memory studies. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 573. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00573
  • Guida, A., Megreya, A. M., Lavielle-Guida, M., Noël, Y., Mathy, F., van Dijck, J.-P., & Abrahamse, E. (2018). Spatialization in working memory is related to literacy and Reading direction: Culture “literarily” directs our thoughts. Cognition, 175, 96–100. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.02.013
  • Guitard, D., & Cowan, N. (2023). The tradeoff between item and order information in short-term memory does not depend on encoding time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 49(1), 51–70. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0001074
  • Healey, M. K., Long, N. M., & Kahana, M. J. (2019). Contiguity in episodic memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26(3), 699–720. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-018-1537-3
  • Hintzman, D. L. (2002). Context matching and judgments of recency. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(2), 368–374. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196295
  • Hintzman, D. L. (2003). Judgments of recency and their relation to recognition memory. Memory & Cognition, 31(1), 26–34. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196079
  • Hintzman, D. L. (2004). Judgment of frequency versus recognition confidence: Repetition and recursive reminding. Memory & Cognition, 32(2), 336–350. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196863
  • Hintzman, D. L. (2005). Memory strength and recency judgments. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12(5), 858–864. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196777
  • Hintzman, D. L. (2010). How does repetition affect memory? Evidence from judgments of recency. Memory & Cognition, 38(1), 102–115. https://doi.org/10.3758/MC.38.1.102
  • Hintzman, D. L. (2016). Is memory organized by temporal contiguity? Memory & Cognition, 44(3), 365–375. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-015-0573-8
  • Hintzman, D. L., & Block, R. A. (1971). Repetition and memory: Evidence for a multiple-trace hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 88(3), 297–306. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0030907
  • Hintzman, D. L., & Block, R. A. (1973). Memory for the spacing of repetitions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 99(1), 70–74. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0034761
  • Hintzman, D. L., Block, R. A., & Summers, J. J. (1973). Contextual associations and memory for serial position. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 97(2), 220–229. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0033884
  • Hitch, G. J., Hurlstone, M. J., & Hartley, T. (2022). Computational models of working memory for language. In J. W. Schweiter & Z. E. Wen (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of working memory and language (pp. 143–174). https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108955638.011
  • Howard, M. W., & Kahana, M. J. (1999). Contextual variability and serial position effects in free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25(4), 923–941. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.25.4.923
  • Howard, M. W., Shankar, K. H., Aue, W. R., & Criss, A. H. (2015). A distributed representation of internal time. Psychological Review, 122(1), 24–53. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037840
  • Howard, M. W., Youker, T. E., & Venkatadass, V. S. (2008). The persistence of memory: Contiguity effects across hundreds of seconds. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(1), 58–63. https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.15.1.58
  • Johnson, M. K., Hashtroudi, S., & Lindsay, D. S. (1993). Source monitoring. Psychological Bulletin, 114(1), 3–28. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.114.1.3
  • Jones, T. C., Armstrong, R., Casey, A., Burson, R. A., & Memon, A. (2013). Verbal description benefits for faces when description conditions are unknown a priori. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(9), 1818–1839. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2013.771688
  • Jones, T. C., & Bartlett, J. C. (2009). When false recognition is out of control: The case of facial conjunctions. Memory & Cognition, 37(2), 143–157. https://doi.org/10.3758/MC.37.2.143
  • Jones, T. C., Robinson, K., & Steel, B. C. (2018). Context retrieval and description benefits for recognition of unfamiliar faces. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 44(10), 1514–1532. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000537
  • Kahana, M. J. (2002). Associative symmetry and memory theory. Memory & Cognition, 30(6), 823–840. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03195769
  • Kahana, M. J., Howard, M. W., Zaromb, F., & Wingfield, A. (2002). Age dissociates recency and lag recency effects in free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28(3), 530–540. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.28.3.530
  • Kanwisher, N., McDermott, J., & Chun, M. M. (1997). The fusiform face area: a module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. The Journal of Neuroscience, 17(11), 4302–4311. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-11-04302.1997
  • Kennerknecht, I., Grueter, T., Welling, B., Wentzek, S., Horst, J., Edwards, S., & Grueter, M. (2006). First report of prevalence of non-syndromic hereditary prosopagnosia (HPA). American Journal of Medical Genetics Part A, 140A(15), 1617–1622. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.a.31343
  • Kerr, N. H., & Winograd, E. (1982). Effects of contextual elaboration on face recognition. Memory & Cognition, 10(6), 603–609. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03202443
  • Klein, K. A., Shiffrin, R. M., & Criss, A. H. (2007). Putting context in context. In J. S. Nairne (Ed.), The foundations of remembering: Essays in honor of Henry L. Roediger, III (pp. 171–189). Psychology Press.
  • Laming, D. (2006). Predicting free recalls. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32(5), 1146–1163. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.32.5.1146
  • Lassen, G. L., Daniel, T. C., & Bartlett, N. R. (1974). Judgment of recency for pictures and words. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 102(5), 795–798. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0036371
  • Logan, G. D., & Cox, G. E. (2023). Serial order depends on item-dependent and item-independent contexts. Psychological Review. https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000422
  • Mensink, G.-J., & Raaijmakers, J. G. (1988). A model for interference and forgetting. Psychological Review, 95(4), 434–455. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.95.4.434
  • Murdock, B. B. (1966). Forward and backward associations in paired associates. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71(5), 732–737. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0023113
  • Murphy, J. S., Wynne, C. E., O’Rourke, E. M., Commins, S., & Roche, R. A. (2009). High-resolution ERP mapping of cortical activation related to implicit object-location memory. Biological Psychology, 82(3), 234–245. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.08.002
  • Ng, H. L. H., & Maybery, M. T. (2002). Grouping in short-term verbal memory: Is position coded temporally? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 55(2), 391–424. https://doi.org/10.1080/02724980143000343
  • Ng, H. L. H., & Maybery, M. T. (2005). Grouping in short-term memory: Do oscillators code the positions of items? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31(1), 175–181. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.31.1.175
  • Osth, A. F., & Hurlstone, M. J. (2023). Do item-dependent context representations underlie serial order in cognition? Commentary on Logan (2021). Psychological Review, 130(2), 513–545. https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000352
  • Potter, M. C., & Levy, E. I. (1969). Recognition memory for a rapid sequence of pictures. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81(1), 10 –15. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0027470
  • Raaijmakers, J. G. W. (2003). Spacing and repetition effects in human memory: Application of the SAM model. Cognitive Science, 27(3), 431–452. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog2703_5
  • Raaijmakers, J. G. W., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1981). Search of associative memory. Psychological Review, 88(2), 93–134. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.88.2.930033-295X.88.2.93
  • Richler, J. J., Tomarken, A. J., Sunday, M. A., Vickery, T. J., Ryan, K. F., Floyd, R. J., Sheinberg, D., Wong, A. C.-N., & Gauthier, I. (2019). Individual differences in object recognition. Psychological Review, 126(2), 226–251. https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000129
  • Rundus, D. (1971). Analysis of rehearsal processes in free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 89(1), 63–77. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0031185
  • Russell, R., Duchaine, B., & Nakayama, K. (2009). Super-recognizers: People with extraordinary face recognition ability. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16(2), 252–257. https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.16.2.252
  • Sahakyan, L., Abushanab, B., Smith, J. R., & Gray, K. J. (2014). Individual differences in contextual storage: Evidence from the list-strength effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40(3), 873–881. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035222
  • Sahakyan, L., & Delaney, P. F. (2005). Directed forgetting in incidental learning and recognition testing: support for a two-factor account. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31(4), 789–801. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.31.4.789
  • Sahakyan, L., & Delaney, P. F. (2010). Item-specific encoding produces an additional benefit of directed forgetting: Evidence from intrusion errors. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36(5), 1346–1354. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020121
  • Sahakyan, L., Delaney, P. F., Foster, N. L., & Abushanab, B. (2013). Psychology of learning and motivation. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 59, 131–189. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-407187-2.00004-6
  • Sahakyan, L., & Kelley, C. M. (2002). A contextual change account of the directed forgetting effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28(6), 1064–1072. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.28.6.1064
  • Sahakyan, L., & Smith, J. R. (2014). ““A long time ago, in a context far, far away”: Retrospective time estimates and internal context change. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40(1), 86–93. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034250
  • Shaffer, W. O., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1972). Rehearsal and storage of visual information. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 92(2), 292–296. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0032076
  • Shiffrin, R. M., & Steyvers, M. (1997). A model for recognition memory: REM—Retrieving effectively from memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4(2), 145–166. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03209391
  • Siegel, L. L., & Kahana, M. J. (2014). A retrieved context account of spacing and repetition effects in free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40(3), 755–764. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035585
  • Smith, S. M., Handy, J. D., Hernandez, A., & Jacoby, L. L. (2018). Context specificity of automatic influences of memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 44(10), 1501–1513. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000523
  • Smith, S. M., & Manzano, I. (2010). Video context-dependent recall. Behavior Research Methods, 42(1), 292–301. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.42.1.292
  • Surprenant, A. M., & Neath, I. (2009). Principles of memory. Taylor & Francis.
  • Tan, L., & Ward, G. (2000). A recency-based account of the primacy effect in free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26(6), 1589–1625. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.26.6.1589
  • Unsworth, N. (2008). Exploring the retrieval dynamics of delayed and final free recall: Further evidence for temporal-contextual search⋆. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(2), 223–236. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2008.04.002
  • Unsworth, N., Brewer, G. A., & Spillers, G. J. (2013). Focusing the search: Proactive and retroactive interference and the dynamics of free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39(6), 1742–1756. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033743
  • Unsworth, N., & Spillers, G. J. (2010). Variation in working memory capacity and episodic recall: The contributions of strategic encoding and contextual retrieval. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17(2), 200 –205. https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.17.2.200
  • van Dijck, J.-P., & Fias, W. (2011). A working memory account for spatial-numerical associations. Cognition, 119(1), 114–119. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2010.12.013
  • Wahlheim, C. N., & Huff, M. J. (2015). Age differences in the focus of retrieval: Evidence from dual-list free recall. Psychology and Aging, 30(4), 768–780. https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000049
  • Wilmer, J. B., Germine, L., Chabris, C. F., Chatterjee, G., Gerbasi, M., & Nakayama, K. (2012). Capturing specific abilities as a window into human individuality: The example of face recognition. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 29(5-6), 360–392. https://doi.org/10.1080/02643294.2012.753433
  • Yntema, D. B., & Trask, F. A. (1963). Recall as a search process. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 2(1), 65–74. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(63)80069-9

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.