331
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Isolating the effects of visual imagery on prospective memory

, ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon
Pages 476-483 | Received 24 Oct 2023, Accepted 19 Mar 2024, Published online: 28 Mar 2024

References

  • Aczel, B., Palfi, B., Szollosi, A., Kovacs, M., Szaszi, B., Szecsi, P., Zrubka, M., Gronau, Q. F., van den Bergh, D., & Wagenmakers, E.-J. (2018). Quantifying support for the null hypothesis in psychology: An empirical investigation. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 1(3), 357–366. https://doi.org/10.1177/2515245918773742
  • Anderson, R. J., Dewhurst, S. A., & Dean, G. M. (2017). Direct and generative retrieval of autobiographical memories: The roles of visual imagery and executive processes. Consciousness and Cognition, 49, 163–171. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2017.02.010
  • Anderson, R. J., Dewhurst, S. A., & Nash, R. A. (2012). Shared cognitive processes underlying past and future thinking: The impact of imagery and concurrent task demands. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38(2), 356–365. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025451
  • Atance, C. M., & O'Neill, D. K. (2001). Episodic future thinking. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5(12), 533–539. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01804-0
  • Baddeley, A. D., & Andrade, J. (2000). Working memory and the vividness of imagery. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129(1), 126–145. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.129.1.126
  • Besken, M., & Mulligan, N. W. (2022). The bizarreness effect and visual imagery: No impact of concurrent visuo-spatial distractor tasks indicates little role for visual imagery. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 48(9), 1281–1295. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001038
  • Block, L. G., & Morwitz, V. G. (1999). Shopping lists as an external memory aid for grocery shopping: Influences on list writing and list fulfilment. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 8, 343–375. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327663jcp0804_01
  • Bower, G. H. (1970). Imagery as a relational organizer in associative learning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 9(5), 529–533. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(70)80096-2
  • Brewer, W. F., & Pani, J. R. (1996). Reports of mental imagery in retrieval from long–term memory. Consciousness and Cognition, 5(3), 265–287. https://doi.org/10.1006/ccog.1996.0019
  • Chasteen, A. L., Park, D. C., & Schwarz, N. (2001). Implementation intentions and facilitation of prospective memory. Psychological Science, 12(6), 457–461. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00385
  • Chen, X., Wang, Y., Liu, L., Cui, J., Gan, M., Shum, D. H., & Chan, R. C. (2015). The effect of implementation intention on prospective memory: A systematic and meta-analytic review. Psychiatry Research, 226(1), 14–22. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2015.01.011
  • Cohen, A.-L., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2008). The cost of remembering to remember: Cognitive load and implementation intentions influence ongoing task performance. In M. Kliegel, M. A. McDaniel, & G. O. Einstein (Eds.), Prospective memory: Cognitive, neuroscience, developmental, and applied perspectives (pp. 367–390). Erlbaum.
  • Conway, M. A., Meares, K., & Standart, S. (2004). Images and goals. Memory (Hove, England), 12(4), 525–531. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210444000151
  • Conway, M. A., & Pleydell-Pearce, C. W. (2000). The construction of autobiographical memories in the self-memory system. Psychological Review, 107(2), 261–288. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.107.2.261
  • D'Argembeau, A., & Van der Linden, M. (2006). Individual differences in the phenomenology of mental time travel: The effect of vivid visual imagery and emotion regulation strategies. Consciousness and Cognition, 15(2), 342–350. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2005.09.001
  • Dewhurst, S. A., & Hitch, G. J. (1999). Cognitive effort and recollective experience in recognition memory. Memory (Hove, England), 7(2), 129–146. https://doi.org/10.1080/741944067
  • Dismukes, R. K. (2012). Prospective memory in workplace and everyday situations. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(4), 215–220. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721412447621
  • Durso, F. T., & Johnson, M. K. (1980). The effects of orienting tasks on recognition, recall, and modality confusion of pictures and words. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19(4), 416–429. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(80)90294-7
  • Einstein, G. O., McDaniel, M. A., Williford, C. L., Pagan, J. L., & Dismukes, R. K. (2003). Forgetting of intentions in demanding situations is rapid. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 9(3), 147–162. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-898X.9.3.147
  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193146
  • Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. American Psychologist, 54(7), 493–503. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.54.7.493
  • Guynn, M. J., McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (1998). Prospective memory: When reminders fail. Memory & Cognition, 26(2), 287–298. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03201140
  • Hauck, P. D., Walsh, C. C., & Kroll, N. E. A. (1976). Visual imagery mnemonics: Common vs. bizarre mental images. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 7(2), 160–162. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03337151
  • Henry, J. D., Terrett, G., Grainger, S. A., Rose, N. S., Kliegel, M., Bugge, M., Ryrie, C., & Rendell, P. G. (2020). Implementation intentions and prospective memory function in late adulthood. Psychology and Aging, 35(8), 1105–1114. https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000563
  • JASP Team. (2024). JASP (Version 0.18. 3) [Computer software].
  • Jeffreys, H. (1961). The theory of probability. Oxford University Press.
  • Klein, S. B., Roberson, T. E., & Delton, A. W. (2011). The future-orientation of memory: Planning as a key component mediating the high levels of recall found with survival processing. Memory (Hove, England), 19(2), 121–139. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2010.537827
  • Kvavilashvili, L., & Rummel, J. (2020). On the nature of everyday prospection: A review and theoretical integration of research on mind-wandering, future thinking, and prospective memory. Review of General Psychology, 24(3), 210–237. https://doi.org/10.1177/1089268020918843
  • Lazzari, R., & Pancheri, P. (1980). Questionario di valutazione dell’ansia di stato e di tratto (State-Trait Anxiety Inventory). Organizzazioni Speciali.
  • Lorayne, H., & Lucas, J. (1974). The memory book. Ballantine.
  • Mahr, J. B. (2020). The dimensions of episodic simulation. Cognition, 196, 104085. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2019.104085
  • Marsh, R L, Hicks, J L, Cook, G I, Hansen, J S, & Pallos, A L. (2003). Interference to ongoing activities covaries with the characteristics of an event-based intention. Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition, 29(5), 861–870. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.29.5.861
  • Marsh, R. L., Hicks, J. L., & Landau, J. D. (1998). An investigation of everyday prospective memory. Memory & Cognition, 26(4), 633–643. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03211383
  • McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (1986). Bizarre imagery as an effective memory aid: The importance of distinctiveness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 12(1), 54–65. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.12.1.54
  • McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (2007). Prospective memory: An overview and synthesis of an emerging field. Sage.
  • McDaniel, M. A., Einstein, G. O., Delosh, E. L., May, C. P., & Brady, P. (1995). The bizarreness effect: It's not surprising, it's complex. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21(2), 422–435. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.21.2.422
  • McDaniel, M. A., Howard, D. C., & Butler, K. M. (2008). Implementation intentions facilitate prospective memory under high attention demands. Memory & Cognition, 36(3), 604–616. https://doi.org/10.3758/MC.36.3.604
  • McFarland, C. P., & Glisky, E. L. (2012). Implementation intentions and imagery: Individual and combined effects on prospective memory among young adults. Memory & Cognition, 40(1), 62–69. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-011-0126-8
  • Meeks, J. T., & Marsh, R. L. (2010). Implementation intentions about nonfocal event-based prospective memory tasks. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 74(1), 82–89. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-008-0223-x
  • Nappe, G. W., & Wollen, K. A. (1973). Effects of instructions to form common and bizarre mental images on retention. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 100(1), 6–8. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0035508
  • Neroni, M. A., Gamboz, N., & Brandimonte, M. A. (2014). Does episodic future thinking improve prospective remembering? Consciousness and Cognition, 23, 53–62. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2013.12.001
  • Paivio, A. (1971). Imagery and verbal processes. Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
  • Richardson, J. T. E. (1975). Concreteness and imageability. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 27(2), 235–249. https://doi.org/10.1080/14640747508400483
  • Rubin, D. C., Schrauf, R. W., & Greenberg, D. L. (2003). Belief and recollection of autobiographical memories. Memory & Cognition, 31(6), 887–901. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196443
  • Rummel, J., & Kvavilashvili, L. (2023). Current theories of prospective memory and new directions for theory development. Nature Reviews Psychology, 2(1), 40–54. https://doi.org/10.1038/s44159-022-00121-4
  • Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., & Buckner, R. L. (2008). Episodic simulation of future events: Concepts, data, and applications. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124(1), 39–60. https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1440.001
  • Scullin, M. K., Kurinec, C. A., & Nguyen, K. (2017). The effects of implementation intention strategies on prospective memory cue encoding. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 29(8), 929–938. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2017.1329205
  • Smith, R. E., & Bayen, U. J. (2004). A multinomial model of event-based prospective memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30(4), 756–777. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.30.4.756
  • Szpunar, K. K., Spreng, R. N., & Schacter, D. L. (2014). A taxonomy of prospection: Introducing an organizational framework for future-oriented cognition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(52), 18414–18421. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1417144111
  • Williams, J. M. G., Healy, H. G., & Ellis, N. C. (1999). The effect of imageability and predicability of cues in autobiographical memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 52(3), 555–579. https://doi.org/10.1080/713755828
  • Zimmermann, T. D., & Meier, B. (2010). The effect of implementation intentions on prospective memory performance across the lifespan. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24(5), 645–658. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1576