Publication Cover
Spatial Cognition & Computation
An Interdisciplinary Journal
Volume 24, 2024 - Issue 2
134
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Am I Crossing a Border? Spatial Regionalization and Route Choice Depends on Perceived Landmark Categorization

&

References

  • Allen, G. L., & Kirasic, K. C. (1985). Effects of the cognitive organization of route knowledge on judgments of macrospatial distance. Memory & Cognition, 13(3), 218–227. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03197684
  • Avraamides, M. N. (2003). Spatial updating of environments described in texts. Cognitive Psychology, 47(4), 402–431. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0285(03)00098-7
  • Avraamides, M. N., & Kelly, J. W. (2008). Multiple systems of spatial memory and action. Cognitive Processing, 9(2), 93–106. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-007-0188-5
  • Avraamides, M. N., Loomis, J. M., Klatzky, R. L., & Golledge, R. G. (2004). Functional equivalence of spatial representations derived from vision and language: Evidence from allocentric judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30(4), 801–814. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.30.4.804
  • Balaguer, J., Spiers, H., Hassabis, D., & Summerfield, C. (2016). Neural mechanisms of hierarchical planning in a virtual subway network. Neuron, 90(4), 893–903. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2016.03.037
  • Belke, B., Leder, H., Harsányi, G., & Carbon, C. C. (2010). When a picasso is a “picasso”: The entry point in the identification of visual art. Acta Psychologica, 133(2), 191–202. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.11.007
  • Bolte, B., Steinicke, F., & Bruder, G. (2011). The jumper metaphor: An effective navigation technique for immersive display setups. Virtual Reality International Conference (Vol. 1, pp. 1–7), 2011.
  • Bowman, D. A., Koller, D., & Hodges, L. F. (1997). Travel in immersive virtual environments: An evaluation of viewpoint motion control techniques. Proceedings of IEEE 1997 Annual International Symposium on Virtual Reality, 45–52.
  • Burris, C. T., & Branscombe, N. R. (2005). Distorted distance estimation induced by a self-relevant national boundary. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41(3), 305–312. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2004.06.012
  • Carbon, C. C., & Hesslinger, V. M. (2013). Attitudes and cognitive distances: On the non-unitary and flexible nature of cognitive maps. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 9(3), 121–129. https://doi.org/10.5709/acp-0140-y
  • Carbon, C. C., & Leder, H. (2005). The wall inside the brain: Overestimation of distances crossing the former iron curtain. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12(4), 746–750. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196767
  • Cherep, L. A., Lim, A. F., Kelly, J. W., Acharya, D., Velasco, A., Bustamante, E., Ostrander, A. G., & Gilbert, S. B. (2020). Spatial cognitive implications of teleporting through virtual environments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 26(3), 480–492. https://doi.org/10.1037/xap0000263
  • Cohen, G. (2000). Hierarchical models in cognition: Do they have psychological reality? European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 12(1), 1–36. https://doi.org/10.1080/095414400382181
  • Collins, R. L., Taylor, S. E., Wood, J. V., & Thompson, S. C. (1988). The vividness effect: Elusive or illusory? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 24(1), 1–18. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031(88)90041-8
  • Couclelis, H., Golledge, R. G., Gale, N., & Tobler, W. (1987). Exploring the anchor-point hypothesis of spatial cognition. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 7(2), 99–122. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0272-4944(87)80020-8
  • Friedman, A., & Brown, N. R. (2000). Reasoning about geography. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Gen- Eral, 129(2), 193–219. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.129.2.193
  • Friedman, A., & Montello, D. R. (2006). Global-scale location and distance estimates: Common representa- tions and strategies in absolute and relative judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32(2), 333–346. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.32.3.333
  • Golledge, R. G., & Spector, A. N. (1978). Comprehending the urban environment: Theory and practice. Geographical Analysis, 10(4), 403–426. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1538-4632.1978.tb00667.x
  • Grassini, S., Laumann, K., de Martin Topranin, V., & Thorp, S. (2021). Evaluating the effect of multi-sensory stimulations on simulator sickness and sense of presence during hmd-mediated vr experience. Ergonomics, 64(12), 1532–1542. https://doi.org/10.1080/00140139.2021.1941279
  • Griesbauer, E.-M., Manley, E., McNamee, D., Morley, J., & Spiers, H. (2022). What determines a boundary for navigating a complex street network: Evidence from london taxi drivers. The Journal of Navigation, 75(1), 15–34. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0373463321000679
  • He, Q., & Brown, T. I. (2019). Environmental barriers disrupt grid-like representations in humans during navi- gation. Current Biology, 29(16), 2718–2722. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2019.06.072
  • Hirtle, S. C., & Jonides, J. (1985). Evidence of hierarchies in cognitive maps. Memory & Cognition, 13(3), 208–217. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03197683
  • Huys, Q. J., Lally, N., Faulkner, P., Eshel, N., Seifritz, E., Gershman, S. J., Dayan, P., & Roiser, J. P. (2015). In- terplay of approximate planning strategies. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(10), 3098–3103. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1414219112
  • Jones, D., Farrand, P., Stuart, G., & Morris, N. (1995). Functional equivalence of verbal and spatial information in serial short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21(4), 1008–1018. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.21.4.1008
  • Klatzky, R. L., Lippa, Y., Loomis, J. M., & Golledge, R. G. (2002). Learning directions of objects specified by vision, spatial audition, or auditory spatial language. Learning & Memory, 9(6), 364–367. https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.51702
  • Klatzky, R. L., Lippa, Y., Loomis, J. M., & Golledge, R. G. (2003). Encoding, learning, and spatial updating of multiple object locations specified by 3-d sound, spatial language, and vision. Experimental Brain Research, 149(1), 48–61. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-002-1334-z
  • Klatzky, R. L., Marston, J. R., Giudice, N. A., Golledge, R. G., & Loomis, J. M. (2006). Cognitive load of nav- igating without vision when guided by virtual sound versus spatial language. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 12(4), 223–232. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-898X.12.4.223
  • Logan, G. D., & Crump, M. J. (2011). Hierarchical control of cognitive processes: The case for skilled type- writing. In Psychology of learning and motivation (pp. 1–27). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-385527-5.00001-2
  • Loomis, J. M., Lippa, Y., Klatzky, R. L., & Golledge, R. G. (2002). Spatial updating of locations specified by 3- d sound and spatial language. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28(2), 335–345. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.28.2.335
  • McNamara, T. P., Hardy, J. K., & Hirtle, S. C. (1989). Subjective hierarchies in spatial memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15(2), 211–227. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.15.2.211
  • Meilinger, T., Strickrodt, M., & Bülthoff, H. H. (2016). Qualitative differences in memory for vista and envi- ronmental spaces are caused by opaque borders, not movement or successive presentation. Cognition, 155, 77–95. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.06.003
  • Mellet, E., Bricogne, S., Crivello, F., Mazoyer, B., Denis, M., & Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. (2002). Neural basis of mental scanning of a topographic representation built from a text. Cerebral Cortex, 12(12), 1322–1330. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/12.12.1322
  • Meneghetti, C., & Pazzaglia, F. (2021). Navigating in virtual environments: Does a map or a map-based de-scription presented beforehand help? Brain Sciences, 11(6), 773. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11060773
  • Montello, D. R. (1992). The geometry of environmental knowledge. In A. Frank, I. Campari, & U. Formentini (Eds.), Theories and methods of spatio-temporal reasoning in geographic space (pp. 136–152). Lecture Notes in Computer Science.
  • Montello, D. R., Fabrikant, S. I., Ruocco, M., & Middleton, R. S. (2003). Testing the first law of cognitive geography on point-display spatializations. Spatial Information Theory. Foundations of Geographic Information Science: International Conference, COSIT 2003, Kartause Ittingen, Switzerland, 316–331. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-540-39923-0_21
  • Newcombe, N., & Liben, L. S. (1982). Barrier effects in the cognitive maps of children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 34(1), 46–58. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0965(82)90030-3
  • Norman, D. A. (1981). Categorization of action slips. Psychological Review, 88(1), 1–15. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.88.1.1
  • Pantelides, S. N., Kelly, J. W., & Avraamides, M. N. (2016). Integration of spatial information across vision and language. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 28(2), 171–185. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2015.1102144
  • Rosch, E., & Mervis, C. B. (1975). Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories. Cognitive Psychology, 7(4), 573–605. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(75)90024-9
  • Rosch, E., Mervis, C. B., Gray, W. D., Johnson, D. M., & Boyes-Braem, P. (1976). Basic objects in natural categories. Cognitive Psychology, 8(3), 382–439. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(76)90013-X
  • Schick, W., Halfmann, M., Hardiess, G., Hamm, F., & Mallot, H. A. (2019). Language cues in the formation of hierarchical representations of space. Spatial Cognition & Computation, 19(3), 252–281. https://doi.org/10.1080/13875868.2019.1576692
  • Smith, S. M., & Shaffer, D. R. (2000). Vividness can undermine or enhance message processing: The moderat- ing role of vividness congruency. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26(7), 769–779. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167200269003
  • Smyth, M. M., & Scholey, K. A. (1996). Serial order in spatial immediate memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 49(1), 159–177. https://doi.org/10.1080/713755615
  • Spiers, H. J., & Maguire, E. A. (2008). The dynamic nature of cognition during wayfinding. Journal of Envi- Ronmental Psychology, 28(3), 232–249. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2008.02.006
  • Stevens, A., & Coupe, P. (1978). Distortions in judged spatial relations. Cognitive Psychology, 10(4), 422–437. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(78)90006-3
  • Tobler, W. R. (1970). A computer movie simulating urban growth in the detroit region. Economic Geography, 46(sup1), 234–240. https://doi.org/10.2307/143141
  • Tom A., & Denis, M. (2003). Referring to landmark or street information in route directions: What difference does it make? COSIT2003 Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 2825, 362–374.
  • Tom A., & Denis, M. (2004). Language and spatial cognition: Comparing the roles of landmarks and street names in route instructions. Applied Cognitive Psychology: The Official Journal of the Society for Applied Research in Memory & Cognition, 18(9), 1213–1230. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1045
  • Tomov, M. S., Yagati, S., Kumar, A., Yang, W., Gershman, S. J., & Pascucci, D. (2020). Discovery of hierarchical representations for efficient planning. PLoS Computational Biology, 16(4), e1007594. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1007594
  • Tom A., & Tversky, B. (2012). Remembering routes: Streets and landmarks. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 26(2), 182–193. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1805
  • Tversky, B. (1981). Distortions in memory for maps. Cognitive Psychology, 13(3), 407–433. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(81)90016-5
  • Tversky, B. (1992). Distortions in cognitive maps. Geoforum, 23(2), 131–138. https://doi.org/10.1016/0016-7185(92)90011-r
  • Tversky, B. (1993). Cognitive maps, cognitive collages and spatial models. In A. Franck & I. Campari (Eds.), Spatial information theory: Theorical basis for GIS (Vol. 716, pp. 14–24). https://doi.org/10.1007/3-540-57207-4_2
  • Uttal, D. H., Friedman, A., Hand, L. L., & Warren, C. (2010). Learning fine-grained and category information in navigable real-world space. Memory & Cognition, 38(8), 1026–1040. https://doi.org/10.3758/MC.38.8.1026
  • Vickery, T. J., & Jiang, Y. V. (2009). Associative grouping: Perceptual grouping of shapes by association. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71(4), 896–909. https://doi.org/10.3758/APP.71.4.896
  • Weech, S., Kenny, S., Lenizky, M., & Barnett-Cowan, M. (2020). Narrative and gaming experience interact to affect presence and cybersickness in virtual reality. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 138, 102398. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijhcs.2020.102398
  • Wiener, J. M., & Mallot, H. A. (2003). ’fine-to-coarse’route planning and navigation in regionalized environ- ments. Spatial Cognition and Computation, 3(4), 331–358. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15427633scc0304_5

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.