253
Views
1
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Infants’ Lexical Processing: Independent Contributions of Attentional and Clarity Cues

ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon

References

  • Adriaans, F., & Swingley, D. (2017). Prosodic exaggeration within infant-directed speech: Consequences for vowel learnability. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 141(5), 3070–3078. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.4982246
  • Audibert, N., & Falk, S. (2018). Vowel space and f0 characteristics of infant-directed singing and speech. In Proceedings of the 19th international conference on speech prosody (pp. 153–157). ISCA.
  • Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1–48. http://dx.doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01
  • Benders, T. (2013). Mommy is only happy! Dutch mothers’ realization of speech sounds in infant-directed speech expresses emotion, not didactic intent. Infant Behavior and Development, 36(4), 847–862. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2013.09.001
  • Benders, T., StGeorge, J., & Fletcher, R. (2021). Infant-directed speech by Dutch fathers: Increased pitch variability within and across utterances. Language Learning and Development, 17(3), 292–325. https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2021.1876698
  • Ben-Shachar, M. S., Lüdecke, D., & Makowski, D. (2020). effectsize: Estimation of effect size indices and standardized parameters. Journal of Open Source Software, 5(56), 2815. https://doi.org/10.21105/joss.02815
  • Bloom, L. (1973). One word at a time: the use of single-word utterances before syntax. The Hague: Mouton. https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110819090
  • Bond, Z. S., & Moore, T. J. (1994). A note on the acoustic-phonetic characteristics of inadvertently clear speech. Speech Communication, 14(4), 325–337. https://doi.org/10.1016/0167-6393(94)90026-4
  • Bradlow, A. R., Torretta, G. M., & Pisoni, D. B. (1996). Intelligibility of normal speech I: Global and fine-grained acoustic-phonetic talker characteristics. Speech Communication, 20(3–4), 255–272. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0167-6393(96)00063-5
  • Broesch, T. L., & Bryant, G. A. (2015). Prosody in infant-directed speech is similar across western and traditional cultures. Journal of Cognition and Development, 16(1), 31–43. https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2013.833923
  • Brookman, R., Kalashnikova, M., Conti, J., Xu Rattanasone, N., Grant, K. A., Demuth, K., & Burnham, D. (2020). Maternal depression affects infants’ lexical processing abilities in the second year of life. Brain Sciences, 10(12), 977. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10120977
  • Bürkner, P. C. (2017). brms: An R package for Bayesian multilevel models using Stan. Journal of Statistical Software, 80(1), 1–28. http://dx.doi.org/10.18637/jss.v080.i01
  • Burnham, D., Kitamura, C., & Vollmer-Conna, U. (2002). What’s new, pussycat? On talking to babies and animals. Science, 296(5572), 1435. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1069587
  • Byrd, D. (1994). Relations of sex and dialect to reduction. Speech Communication, 15(1–2), 39–54. https://doi.org/10.1016/0167-6393(94)90039-6
  • Castellanos, A., Benedí, J. M., & Casacuberta, F. (1996). An analysis of general acoustic-phonetic features for Spanish speech produced with the Lombard effect. Speech Communication, 20(1–2), 23–35. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0167-6393(96)00042-8
  • Cooper, R. P., & Aslin, R. N. (1990). Preference for infant‐directed speech in the first month after birth. Child Development, 61(5), 1584–1595. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1990.tb02885.x
  • Cox, C., Bergmann, C., Fowler, E., Keren-Portnoy, T., Roepstorff, A., Bryant, G., & Fusaroli, R. (2022a). A systematic review and bayesian meta-analysis of the acoustic features of infant-directed speech. Nature Human Behaviour. https://doi.org/10.21203/rs.3.rs-1396005/v1
  • Cox, C. M. M., Dideriksen, C., Keren-Portnoy, T., Roepstorff, A., Christiansen, M. H., & Fusaroli, R. (2022b). Infant-directed speech does not always involve exaggerated vowel distinctions: Evidence from Danish. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/2gswt
  • Cristia, A., Dupoux, E., Gurven, M., & Stieglitz, J. (2019). Child‐directed speech is infrequent in a forager‐farmer population: A time allocation study. Child Development, 90(3), 759–773. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12974
  • Cristia, A., & Seidl, A. (2014). The hyperarticulation hypothesis of infant-directed speech. Journal of Child Language, 41(4), 913–934. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000912000669
  • Dink, J. W., & Ferguson, B. (2015). eyetrackingR: An R library for eye-tracking data analysis. www.eyetracking-r.com
  • Dunst, C., Gorman, E., & Hamby, D. (2012). Preference for infant-directed speech in preverbal young children. Center for Early Literacy Learning, 5(1), 1–13.
  • Englund, K. T. (2018). Hypoarticulation in infant-directed speech. Applied Psycholinguistics, 39(1), 67–87. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716417000480
  • Englund, K., & Behne, D. (2006). Changes in infant directed speech in the first six months. Infant and Child Development: An International Journal of Research and Practice, 15(2), 139–160. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.445
  • Farran, L. K., Lee, C. C., Yoo, H., & Oller, D. K. (2016). Cross-cultural register differences in infant-directed speech: An initial study. PloS one, 11(3), e0151518. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0151518
  • Fenson, L., Dale, P., Reznick, J. S., Thal, D., Bates, E., Hartung, J., & Reilly, J. (1993). MacArthur Communicative Inventories: User’s guide and technical manual. San Diego.
  • Ferguson, S. H., & Kewley-Port, D. (2007). Talker Differences in Clear and Conversational Speech: Acoustic Characteristics of Vowels. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 1241–1255. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2007/087)
  • Fernald, A. (1989). Intonation and communicative intent in mothers' speech to infants: is the melody the message?Child Development. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130938
  • Fernald, A. (1993). Approval and disapproval: Infant responsiveness to vocal affect in familiar and unfamiliar languages. Child Development, 64(3), 657–674. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1993.tb02934.x
  • Fernald, A. (2000). Speech to infants as hyperspeech: Knowledge-driven processes in early word recognition. Phonetica, 57(2–4), 242–254. https://doi.org/10.1159/000028477
  • Fernald, A., & Marchman, V. A. (2012). Individual differences in lexical processing at 18 months predict vocabulary growth in typically developing and late‐talking toddlers. Child Development, 83(1), 203–222. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01692.x
  • Fernald, A., Marchman, V. A., & Weisleder, A. (2013). SES differences in language processing skill and vocabulary are evident at 18 months. Developmental Science, 16(2), 234–248. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12019
  • Fernald, A., Perfors, A., & Marchman, V. A. (2006). Picking up speed in understanding: Speech processing efficiency and vocabulary growth across the 2nd year. Developmental Psychology, 42(1), 98. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0012-1649.42.1.98
  • Fernald, A., & Simon, T. (1984). Expanded intonation contours in mothers’ speech to newborns. Developmental Psychology, 20(1), 104. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.20.1.104
  • Fernald, A., Swingley, D., & Pinto, J. P. (2001). When half a word is enough: Infants can recognize spoken words using partial phonetic information. Child Development, 72(4), 1003–1015. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00331
  • Fernald, A., Zangl, R., Portillo, A. L., & Marchman, V. A. (2008). Looking while listening: Using eye movements to monitor spoken language. Developmental Psycholinguistics: On-line Methods in CHILDREN’S LANGUAGE PROCESSING, 44, 97.
  • Frank, M. C., Braginsky, M., Yurovsky, D., & Marchman, V. A. (2016). Wordbank: An open repository for developmental vocabulary data. Journal of Child Language, 1–18. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000916000209
  • Garrison, H., Baudet, G., Breitfeld, E., Aberman, A., & Bergelson, E. (2020). Familiarity plays a small role in noun comprehension at 12–18 months. Infancy, 25(4), 458–477. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12333
  • Gergely, A., Faragó, T., Galambos, Á., & Topál, J. (2017). Differential effects of speech situations on mothers’ and fathers’ infant-directed and dog-directed speech: An acoustic analysis. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-13883-2
  • Graf Estes, K., & Hurley, K. (2013). Infant‐directed prosody helps infants map sounds to meanings. Infancy, 18(5), 797–824. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12006
  • Grieser, D. L., & Kuhl, P. K. (1988). Maternal speech to infants in a tonal language: Support for universal prosodic features in motherese. Developmental Psychology, 24(1), 14. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0012-1649.24.1.14
  • Háden, G. P., Mády, K., Török, M., & Winkler, I. (2020). Newborn infants differently process adult directed and infant directed speech. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 147, 107–112. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2019.10.011
  • Han, M., De Jong, N., & Kager, R. (2020). Pitch properties of infant-directed speech specific to word-learning contexts: A cross-linguistic investigation of Mandarin Chinese and Dutch. Journal of Child Language, 47(1), 85–111. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000919000813
  • Hartman, K. M., Ratner, N. B., & Newman, R. S. (2017). Infant-directed speech (IDS) vowel clarity and child language outcomes. Journal of Child Language, 44(5), 1140–1162. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000916000520
  • Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Paul H Brookes Publishing.
  • Hazan, V., & Markham, D. (2004). Acoustic-phonetic correlates of talker intelligibility for adults and children. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 116(5), 3108–3118. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.1806826
  • Hilton, C. B., Moser, C. J., Bertolo, M., Lee-Rubin, H., Amir, D., Bainbridge, C. M., … Mehr, S. A. (2022). Acoustic regularities in infant-directed speech and song across cultures. Nature Human Behaviour, 1–12.
  • Kalashnikova, M., & Burnham, D. (2018). Infant-directed speech from seven to nineteen months has similar acoustic properties but different functions. Journal of Child Language, 1–19. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000917000629
  • Kalashnikova, M., Carignan, C., & Burnham, D. (2017). The origins of babytalk: Smiling, teaching or social convergence? Royal Society Open Science, 4(8), 170306. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.170306
  • Kalashnikova, M., & Carreiras, M. (2022). Input quality and speech perception development in bilingual infants’ first year of life. Child Development, 93(1), e32–e46. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13686
  • Kalashnikova, M., Schwarz, I. C., & Burnham, D. (2016). OZI: Australian English communicative development inventory. First Language, 36(4), 407–427. https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723716648846
  • Kaplan, P. S., Jung, P. C., Ryther, J. S., & Zarlengo-Strouse, P. (1996). Infant-directed versus adult-directed speech as signals for faces. Developmental Psychology, 32(5), 880. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.32.5.880
  • Kitamura, C., & Notley, A. (2009). The shift in infant preferences for vowel duration and pitch contour between 6 and 10 months of age. Developmental Science, 12(5), 706–714. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00818.x
  • Kitamura, C., Thanavishuth, C., Burnham, D., & Luksaneeyanawin, S. (2001). Universality and specificity in infant-directed speech: Pitch modifications as a function of infant age and sex in a tonal and non-tonal language. Infant Behavior and Development, 24(4), 372–392. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(02)00086-3
  • Kuhl, P. K. (2000). A new view of language acquisition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 97(22), 11850–11857. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.97.22.11850
  • Kuhl, P. K., Andruski, J. E., Chistovich, I. A., Chistovich, L. A., Kozhevnikova, E. V., Ryskina, V. L., Stolyarova, E. I., Sundberg, U., & Lacerda, F. (1997). Cross-language analysis of phonetic units in language addressed to infants. Science, 277(5326), 684–686. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.277.5326.684
  • Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. (2017). lmerTest package: Tests in linear mixed effects models. Journal of Statistical Software, 82(13), 1–26. https://doi.org/10.18637/JSS.V082.I13
  • Lam, J., Tjaden, K., & Wilding, G. (2012). Acoustics of clear speech: Effect of instruction. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 55(6), 1807–1821. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092–4388(2012/11-0154)
  • Liu, H. M., Kuhl, P. K., & Tsao, F. M. (2003). An association between mothers’ speech clarity and infants’ speech discrimination skills. Developmental Science, 6(3), F1–F10. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-7687.00275
  • Lorge, I., & Katsos, N. (2019). Listener-adapted speech: Bilinguals adapt in a more sensitive way. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 9(3), 376–397. https://doi.org/10.1075/lab.16054.lor
  • Lovcevic, I., Kalashnikova, M., & Burnham, D. (2020). Acoustic features of infant-directed speech to infants with hearing loss. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 148(6), 3399–3416. https://doi.org/10.1121/10.0002641
  • Ma, W., Golinkoff, R. M., Houston, D. M., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2011). Word learning in infant-and adult-directed speech. Language Learning and Development, 7(3), 185–201. https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2011.579839
  • ManyBabies Consortium. (2020). Quantifying sources of variability in infancy research using the infant-directed-speech preference. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 3(1), 24–52. https://doi.org/10.1177/2515245919900809
  • Marchman, V. A., & Fernald, A. (2008). Speed of word recognition and vocabulary knowledge in infancy predict cognitive and language outcomes in later childhood. Developmental Science, 11(3), F9–F16. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00671.x
  • Marklund, E., Marklund, U., & Gustavsson, L. (2021). An association between phonetic complexity of infant vocalizations and parent vowel hyperarticulation. Frontiers in Psychology, 2873. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.693866
  • Martin, A., Schatz, T., Versteegh, M., Miyazawa, K., Mazuka, R., Dupoux, E., & Cristia, A. (2015). Mothers speak less clearly to infants than to adults: A comprehensive test of the hyperarticulation hypothesis. Psychological Science, 26(3), 341–347. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614562453
  • McMurray, B., Kovack-Lesh, K. A., Goodwin, D., & McEchron, W. (2013). Infant directed speech and the development of speech perception: Enhancing development or an unintended consequence? Cognition, 129(2), 362–378. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.07.015
  • Menn, K. H., Michel, C., Meyer, L., Hoehl, S., & Männel, C. (2022). Natural infant-directed speech facilitates neural tracking of prosody. NeuroImage, 251, 118991. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.118991
  • Miyazawa, K., Shinya, T., Martin, A., Kikuchi, H., & Mazuka, R. (2017). Vowels in infant-directed speech: More breathy and more variable, but not clearer. Cognition, 166, 84–93. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2017.05.003
  • Moon, S. J., & Lindblom, B. (1994). Interaction between duration, context, and speaking style in English stressed vowels. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 96(1), 40–55. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.410492
  • Nakamura, M., Iwano, K., & Furui, S. (2008). Differences between acoustic characteristics of spontaneous and read speech and their effects on speech recognition performance. Computer Speech & Language, 22(2), 171–184. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.csl.2007.07.003
  • Naoi, N., Minagawa-Kawai, Y., Kobayashi, A., Takeuchi, K., Nakamura, K., Yamamoto, J. I., & Shozo, K. (2012). Cerebral responses to infant-directed speech and the effect of talker familiarity. Neuroimage, 59(2), 1735–1744. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.093
  • Narayan, C. R., & McDermott, L. C. (2016). Speech rate and pitch characteristics of infant-directed speech: Longitudinal and cross-linguistic observations. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 139(3), 1272–1281. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.4944634
  • Panneton, R., Kitamura, C., Mattock, K., & Burnham, D. (2006). Slow speech enhances younger but not older infants’ perception of vocal emotion. Research in Human Development, 3(1), 7–19. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15427617rhd0301_2
  • Peter, V., Kalashnikova, M., Santos, A., & Burnham, D. (2016). Mature neural responses to infant-directed speech but not adult-directed speech in pre-verbal infants. Scientific Reports, 6(1), 34273. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep34273
  • Piazza, G., Martin, C. D., & Kalashnikova, M. (2021). The acoustic features and didactic function of Foreigner directed speech: A literature review.
  • Ratner, N. B., & Pye, C. (1984). Higher pitch in BT is not universal: Acoustic evidence from Quiche Mayan. Journal of Child Language, 11(3), 515–522. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900005924
  • Rattanasone, N. X., Burnham, D., & Reilly, R. G. (2013). Tone and vowel enhancement in Cantonese infant-directed speech at 3, 6, 9, and 12 months of age. Journal of Phonetics, 41(5), 332–343. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2013.06.001
  • R Core Team. (2020). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. https://www.r-project.org/
  • Ronfard, S., Ran, W. E. I., & Rowe, L. M. (2022). Exploring the linguistic, cognitive, and social skills underlying lexical processing efficiency as measured by the looking-while-listening paradigm. Journal of Child Language, 49(2), 302–325. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000921000106
  • Saito, Y., Aoyama, S., Kondo, T., Fukumoto, R., Konishi, N., Nakamura, K., … Toshima, T. (2007). Frontal cerebral blood flow change associated with infant-directed speech. Archives of Disease in Childhood-Fetal and Neonatal Edition, 92(2), F113–F116. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/adc.2006.097949
  • Santesso, D. L., Schmidt, L. A., & Trainor, L. J. (2007). Frontal brain electrical activity (EEG) and heart rate in response to affective infant-directed (ID) speech in 9-month-old infants. Brain and Cognition, 65(1), 14–21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2007.02.008
  • Shneidman, L. A., & Goldin‐Meadow, S. (2012). Language input and acquisition in a Mayan village: How important is directed speech? Developmental Science, 15(5), 659–673. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.14677687.2012.01168.x
  • Smiljanić, R., & Bradlow, A. R. (2005). Production and perception of clear speech in Croatian and English. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 118(3), 1677–1688. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.2000788
  • Song, J. Y., Demuth, K., & Morgan, J. (2010). Effects of the acoustic properties of infant-directed speech on infant word recognition. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 128(1), 389–400. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.3419786
  • Spinelli, M., Fasolo, M., & Mesman, J. (2017). Does prosody make the difference? A meta-analysis on relations between prosodic aspects of infant-directed speech and infant outcomes. Developmental Review, 44, 1–18. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2016.12.001
  • Spinelli, M., & Mesman, J. (2018). The regulation of infant negative emotions: The role of maternal sensitivity and infant‐directed speech prosody. Infancy, 23(4), 502–518. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12237
  • Stern, D. N., Spieker, S., & MacKain, K. (1982). Intonation contours as signals in maternal speech to prelinguistic infants. Developmental Psychology, 18(5), 727. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0012-1649.18.5.727
  • Suttora, C., Salerni, N., Zanchi, P., Zampini, L., Spinelli, M., & Fasolo, M. (2017). Relationships between structural and acoustic properties of maternal talk and children’s early word recognition. First Language, 37(6), 612–629. https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723717714946
  • Swingley, D., & Aslin, R. N. (2000). Spoken word recognition and lexical representation in very young children. Cognition, 76(2), 147–166. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00081-0
  • Swingley, D., & Aslin, R. N. (2002). Lexical neighborhoods and the word-form representations of 14-month-olds. Psychological Science, 13(5), 480–484. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00485
  • Tang, P., Xu Rattanasone, N., Yuen, I., & Demuth, K. (2017). Phonetic enhancement of Mandarin vowels and tones: Infant-directed speech and Lombard speech. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 142(2), 493–503. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.4995998
  • Thiessen, E. D., Hill, E. A., & Saffran, J. R. (2005). Infant-directed speech facilitates word segmentation. Infancy, 7(1), 53–71. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327078in0701_5
  • Trainor, L. J., Austin, C. M., & Desjardins, R. N. (2000). Is infant-directed speech prosody a result of the vocal expression of emotion? Psychological Science, 11(3), 188–195. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00240
  • Trainor, L. J., & Desjardins, R. N. (2002). Pitch characteristics of infant-directed speech affect infants’ ability to discriminate vowels. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(2), 335–340. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196290
  • Uther, M., Knoll, M. A., & Burnham, D. (2007). Do you speak E-NG-LI-SH? A comparison of foreigner-and infant-directed speech. Speech communication, 49(1), 2–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.specom.2006.10.003
  • Van der Feest, S. V., Blanco, C. P., & Smiljanic, R. (2019). Influence of speaking style adaptations and semantic context on the time course of word recognition in quiet and in noise. Journal of Phonetics, 73, 158–177. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2019.01.003
  • Wang, L., Kalashnikova, M., Kager, R., Lai, R., & Wong, P. (2021). Lexical and prosodic pitch modifications in cantonese infant-directed speech. Journal of Child Language, 48(6), 1235–1261. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000920000707
  • Warlaumont, A. S., Richards, J. A., Gilkerson, J., & Oller, D. K. (2014). A social feedback loop for speech development and its reduction in autism. Psychological Science, 25(7), 1314–1324. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614531023
  • Weber, A., Fernald, A., & Diop, Y. (2017). When cultural norms discourage talking to babies: Effectiveness of a parenting program in rural Senegal. Child Development, 88(5), 1513–1526. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12882
  • Weirich, M., & Simpson, A. (2019). Effects of gender, parental role, and time on infant-and adult-directed read and spontaneous speech. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 62(11), 4001–4014. https://doi.org/10.1044/2019_JSLHR-S-19-0047
  • Xu, N., Burnham, D., Kitamura, C., & Vollmer-Conna, U. (2013). Vowel hyperarticulation in parrot-, dog-and infant-directed speech. Anthrozoös, 26(3), 373–380. https://doi.org/10.2752/175303713X13697429463592
  • Zangl, R., Klarman, L., Thal, D., Fernald, A., & Bates, E. (2005). Dynamics of word comprehension in infancy: Developments in timing, accuracy, and resistance to acoustic degradation. Journal of Cognition and Development, 6(2), 179–208. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327647jcd0602_2
  • Zangl, R., & Mills, D. L. (2007). Increased brain activity to infant‐directed speech in 6‐and 13‐month‐old infants. Infancy, 11(1), 31–62. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327078in1101_2
  • Zhang, Y., Koerner, T., Miller, S., Grice‐Patil, Z., Svec, A., Akbari, D., Tusler, L., & Carney, E. (2011). Neural coding of formant‐exaggerated speech in the infant brain. Developmental Science, 14(3), 566–581. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.01004.x

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.