183
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Articles

Do learning disabilities in reading, spelling and numeracy have common underlying factors? Evidence from Arabic-speaking children sample

ORCID Icon, &

References

  • Abu-Rabia, S. (1999). The effect of Arabic vowels on the reading comprehension of second- and sixth-grade native Arab children. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 28(1), 93–101. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023291620997
  • Abu-Rabia, S. (2001). The role of vowels in reading Semitic scripts: Data from Arabic and Hebrew. Reading and writing. An Interdisciplinary Journal, 14, 39–59. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1008147606320
  • Abu-Rabia, S., & Siegel, L. S. (2002). Reading, syntactic, orthographic, and working memory skills of bilingual Arabic–English speaking Canadian children. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 31(6), 661–678. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1021221206119
  • American Psychiatric Association (APA). (1994). (4th ed.). American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc.
  • American Psychiatric Association, DSM-5 Task Force. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5™ (5th ed.). American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc.
  • Anthony, J. L., & Francis, D. J. (2005). Development of phonological awareness. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14(5), 255–259. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00376.x
  • Apel, K., & Lawrence, J. (2011). Contributions of morphological awareness skills to word-level reading and spelling in first-grade children with and without speech sound disorder. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 54(5), 1312–1327. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2011/10-0115)
  • Asadi, I. A., Khateb, A., Ibrahim, R., & Taha, H. (2017). How do different cognitive and linguistic variables contribute to reading in Arabic? A cross-sectional study from first to sixth grade. Reading and Writing, 30(9), 1835–1867. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-017-9755-z
  • Boudelaa, S., & Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (2001). Morphological units in the Arabic mental lexicon. Cognition, 81(1), 65–92. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(01)00119-6
  • Bouma, A., Mulder, J., & Lindeboom, J. (1996). Neuropsycholo-gische diagnostiek: Handbook [Neuropsychological assessment: Manual]. Swets & Zeitlinger.
  • Butterworth, B. (2010). Foundational numerical capacities and the origins of dyscalculia. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(12), 534–541. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.09.007
  • Butterworth, B., & Laurillard, D. (2010). Low numeracy and dyscalculia: Identification and intervention. ZDM Mathematics Education, 42(6), 527–539. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11858-010-0267-4
  • Capodieci, A., Serafini, A., Dessuki, A., & Cornoldi, C. (2019). Writing abilities and the role of working memory in children with symptoms of attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder. Child Neuropsychology, 25(1), 103–121. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2018.1441390
  • Catts, H. W., Hogan, T. P., & Fey, M. E. (2003). Subgrouping poor readers on the basis of individual differences in reading-related abilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 36(2), 151–164.
  • Chinn, S. (2015). The Routledge international handbook of dyscalculia and mathematical learning difficulties. Routledge.
  • Couture, A. E., & Mccauley, R. J. (2000). Phonological working memory in children with phonological impairment. Clinical Linguistic and Phonetics, 14, 499–517.
  • Davidse, N. J., De Jong, M. T., & Bus, A. G. (2014). Explaining common variance shared by early numeracy and literacy. Reading and Writing, 27(4), 631–648.
  • De Smedt, B., & Boets, B. (2010). Phonological processing and arithmetic fact retrieval: Evidence from developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 48(14), 3973–3981.
  • De Smedt, B., Taylor, J., Archibald, L., & Ansari, D. (2010). How is phonological processing related to individual differences in children’s arithmetic skills? Developmental Science, 13(3), 508–520. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00897.x
  • Deacon, S. H., Benere, J., & Castles, A. (2012). Chicken or egg? Untangling the relationship between orthographic processing skill and reading accuracy. Cognition, 122(1), 110–117. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2011.09.003
  • Dirks, E., Spyer, G., Van Lieshout, E. C., & de Sonneville, L. (2008). Prevalence of combined reading and arithmetic disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 41(5), 460–473. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219408321128
  • Döhla, D., & Heim, S. (2016). Developmental dyslexia and dysgraphia: What can we learn from the one about the other? Frontiers in Psychology, 6, Article 2045. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.02045
  • Döhla, D., Willmes, K., & Heim, S. (2018). Cognitive profiles of developmental dysgraphia. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, Article 2006. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02006
  • Dotan, D., & Friedmann, N. (2019). Reducing interference improves the memorization of multiplication facts in case of hypersensitivity to interference. Journal of Numerical Cognition, 5(3), 400–430. https://doi.org/10.5964/jnc.v5i3.203
  • Dowker, A. D. (2004). Children with difficulties in mathematics: What works? DfES.
  • Ehri, L. C. (2005). Development of sight word reading: Phases and findings. In M. J. Snowling & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 135–154). Blackwell Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470757642.ch8
  • Furnes, B., & Samuelsson, S. (2011). Phonological awareness and rapid automatized naming predicting early development in reading and spelling: Results from a Cross-linguistic longitudinal study. Learning and Individual Differences, 21(1), 85–95.
  • Geary, D. C. (2013). Early foundations for mathematics learning and their relations to learning disabilities. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(1), 23–27. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721412469398
  • Geary, D. C., & Hoard, M. K. (2001). Numerical and arithmetical deficits in learning-disabled children: Relation to dyscalculia and dyslexia. Aphasiology, 15(7), 635–647. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687040143000113
  • Georgiou, G. K., Torppa, M., Manolitsis, G., Lyytinen, H., & Parrila, R. (2012). Longitudinal predictors of reading and spelling across languages varying in orthographic consistency. Reading and Writing, 25(2), 321–346. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-010-9271-x
  • Georgiou, G. K., Tziraki, N., Manolitsis, G., & Fella, A. (2013). Is rapid automatized naming related to reading and mathematics for the same reason(s)? A follow-up study from kindergarten to Grade 1. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 115(3), 481–496.
  • Goodrich, J. M., Lonigan, C. J., & Farver, J. M. (2013). Do early literacy skills in children’s first language promote development of skills in their second language? An experimental evaluation of transfer. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105(2), 414–426. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031780
  • Grainger, J., & Hannagan, T. (2014). What is special about orthographic processing? Written Language & Literacy, 17(2), 225–252. https://doi.org/10.1075/wll.17.2.03gra
  • Holloway, I. D., & Ansari, D. (2009). Mapping numerical magnitudes onto symbols: The numerical distance effect and individual differences in children’s mathematics achievement. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103(1), 17–29.
  • Hulme, C., & Snowling, M. J. (2016). Reading disorders and dyslexia. Current Opinion in Pediatrics, 28(6), 731–735.
  • Jordan, J. A., Wylie, J., & Mulhern, G. (2015). Mathematics and reading difficulty subtypes: Minor phonological influences on mathematics for 5-7-years-old. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 221. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00221
  • Kargiotidis, A., Grigorakis, I., Mouzaki, A., & Manolitsis, G. (2021). Differences in oral language growth between children with and without literacy difficulties: Evidence from early phases of learning to read and spell in Greek. Australian Journal of Learning Difficulties, 26(2), 89–112. https://doi.org/10.1080/19404158.2021.1961160
  • Kirby, J. R., Parrila, R. K., & Pfeiffer, S. L. (2003). Naming speed and phonological awareness as predictors of reading development. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(3), 453–464. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.95.3.453
  • Krajewski, K., & Schneider, W. (2009). Exploring the impact of phonological awareness, visual-spatial working memory, and preschool quantity-number competencies on mathematics achievement in elementary school: Findings from a 3-year longitudinal study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103(4), 516–531.
  • Landerl, K., & Wimmer, H. (2000). Deficits in phoneme segmentation are not the core problem of dyslexia: Evidence from German and English children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21(2), 243–262. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716400002058
  • Landerl, K., & Wimmer, H. (2008). Development of word reading fluency and spelling in a consistent orthography: An 8-year follow-up. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(1), 150–161. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.100.1.150
  • Layes, L., Tibi, S., Bouakkaz, Y., & Rebai, M. (2022). Phonological awareness deficits in children with dyslexia: The impact of working memory as a function of modality of test administration. Reading & Writing Quarterly, 38(2), 184–197. https://doi.org/10.1080/10573569.2021.1936712
  • Layes, S., Chouchani, M. S., Mecheri, S., Lalonde, R., & Rebaï, M. (2019). Efficacy of a visuomotor-based intervention for children with reading and spelling disabilities: A pilot study. British Journal of Special Education, 46(3), 317–339. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8578.12278
  • Martin, R. B., Cirino, P. T., Sharp, C., & Barnes, M. (2014). Number and counting skills in kindergarten as predictors of grade 1 mathematical skills. Learning and Individual Differences, 34, 12–23.
  • Melby-Lervåg, M., & Hulme, C. (2010). Serial and free recall in children can be improved by training: Evidence for the importance of phonological and semantic representations in immediate memory tasks. Psychological Science, 21(11), 1694–1700. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610385355
  • Moll, K., Göbel, S. M., Gooch, D., Landerl, K., & Snowling, M. J. (2016). Cognitive risk factors for specific learning disorder: Processing speed, temporal processing, and working memory. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 49(3), 272–281.
  • Moll, K., Kunze, S., Neuhoff, N., Bruder, J., & Schulte-Körne, G. (2014). Specific learning disorder: Prevalence and gender differences. PLoS One, 9(7), e103537.
  • Moura, O., Simões, M. R., & Pereira, M. (2015). Executive functioning in children with developmental dyslexia. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 28(Suppl 1), S20–S41. https://doi.org/10.1080/13854046.2014.964326
  • Nevo, E., & Breznitz, Z. (2013). The development of working memory from kindergarten to first grade in children with different decoding skills. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114(2), 217–228.
  • Olson, R., Forsberg, H., Wise, B., & Rack, J. (1994). Measurement of word recognition, orthographic, and phonological skills. In G. R. Lyon (Ed.), Frames of reference for the assessment of learning disabilities: New views on measurement issues (p. 243–277). Paul H Brookes Publishing Co.
  • Papadopoulos, T. C., Georgiou, G. K., & Kendeou, P. (2009). Investigating the double-deficit hypothesis in Greek. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 42(6), 528–547.
  • Pennington, B. F. (2006). From single to multiple deficit models of developmental disorders. Cognition, 101(2), 385–413. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2006.04.008
  • Peters, L., & Ansari, D. (2019). Are specific learning disorders truly specific, and are they disorders? Trends in Neuroscience and Education, 17, 100115–100115. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tine.2019.100115
  • PIAAC Numeracy Expert Group. (2009). PIAAC numeracy: A conceptual framework.documents de travail de l‘OCDE sur l‘éducation, n° 35, Éditions OCDE.
  • Powell, S. R., Fuchs, L. S., & Fuchs, D. (2013). Reaching the mountaintop: Addressing the common core standards in mathematics for students with mathematics difficulties. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 28(1), 38–48. https://doi.org/10.1111/ldrp.12001
  • Puranik, C. S., & Alotaiba, S. (2012). Examining the contribution of handwriting and spelling to written expression in kindergarten children. Reading and Writing, 25(7), 1523–1546.
  • Riva, D., Nichelli, F., & Devoti, M. (2000). Developmental aspects of verbal fluency and confrontation naming in children. Brain and Language, 71(2), 267–284.
  • Romano, E., Babchishin, L., Pagani, L. S., & Kohen, D. (2010). School readiness and later achievement: Replication and extension using a nationwide Canadian survey. Developmental Psychology, 46(5), 995–1007.
  • Seigneuric, A., & Ehrlich, M. (2005). Contribution of working memory capacity to children’s reading comprehension: A longitudinal investigation. Reading and Writing, 18(7-9), 617–656. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-005-2038-0
  • Share, D. L. (2008). On the Anglocentricities of current reading research and practice: The perils of overreliance on an "outlier" orthography. Psychological Bulletin, 134(4), 584–615. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.4.584
  • Simmons, F. R., & Singleton, C. (2008). Do weak phonological representations impact on arithmetic development? A review of research into arithmetic and dyslexia. Dyslexia, 14(2), 77–94.
  • Slot, E. M., van Viersen, S., de Bree, E. H., & Kroesbergen, E. H. (2016). Shared and unique risk factors underlying mathematical disability and reading and spelling disability. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 803.
  • Snowling, M. J., Moll, K., & Hulme, C. (2021). Language difficulties are a shared risk factor for both reading disorder and mathematics disorder. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 202, 105009. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2020.105009
  • Stone, B., & Brady, S. (1995). Evidence for deficits in basic phonological processes in less-skilled readers. Annals of Dyslexia, 45(1), 51–78. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02648212
  • Strauss, E., Sherman, E. M. S., & Spreen, O. (2006). A compendium of neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms, and commentary (3rd ed.). Oxford University Press.
  • Tibi, S., & Kirby, J. R. (2019). Reading in Arabic: How well does the standard model apply? Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 62(4), 993–1014. https://doi.org/10.1044/2019_JSLHR-L-18-0193
  • Tibi, S., Fitton, L., & McIlraith, A. L. (2021). The development of a measure of orthographic knowledge in the Arabic language: A psychometric evaluation. Applied Psycholinguistics, 42(3), 739–762. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716421000035
  • Tong, X., Deacon, S. H., Kirby, J. R., Cain, K., & Parrila, R. (2011). Morphological awareness: A key to understanding poor reading comprehension in English. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103(3), 523–534. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023495
  • Torppa, M., Georgiou, G. K., Niemi, P., Lerkkanen, M.-K., & Poikkeus, A.-M. (2017). The precursors of double dissociation between reading and spelling in a transparent orthography. Annals of Dyslexia, 67(1), 42–62. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-016-0131-5
  • Vellutino, F. R., Fletcher, J. M., Snowling, M. J., & Scanlon, D. M. (2004). Specific reading disability (dyslexia): What have we learned in the past four decades? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 45(1), 2–40.
  • Von Aster, M. (2001). Die neuropsychologischeTestbatterie für Zahlenverarbeitung und RechnenbeiKindern (ZAREKI) [The neuropsychological test battery for number processing and calculation in children (NUCALC)]. Swets&Zeitliger.
  • Willburger, E., Fussenegger, B., Moll, K., Wood, G., & Landerl, K. (2008). Naming speed in dyslexia and dyscalculia. Learning and Individual Differences, 18(2), 224–236. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2008.01.003
  • Willcutt, E. G., Pennington, B. F., Olson, R. K., Chhabildas, N., & Hulslander, J. (2005). Neuropsychological analyses of comorbidity between reading disability and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: In search of the common deficit. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27(1), 35–78. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326942dn2701_3
  • Wilson, A. J., Andrewes, S. G., Struthers, H., Rowe, V. M., Bogdanovic, R., & Waldie, K. E. (2015). Dyscalculia and dyslexia in adults: Cognitive bases of comorbidity. Learning and Individual Differences, 37, 118–132. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2014.11.017
  • Zarić, J., Hasselhorn, M., & Nagler, T. (2021). Orthographic knowledge predicts reading and spelling skills over and above general intelligence and phonological awareness. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 36(1), 21–43. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10212-020-00464-7
  • Ziegler, J. C., Perry, C., Ma-Wyatt, A., Ladner, D., & Schulte-Körne, G. (2003). Developmental dyslexia in different languages: Language-specific or universal? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 86(3), 169–193. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-0965(03)00139-5

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.