34
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Chinese Buddhism and Vertical Space Metaphoric ThinkingOpen Data

ORCID Icon, , , &

References

  • Arcuri, L., Castelli, L., Galdi, S., Zogmaister, C., & Amadori, A. (2008). Predicting the vote: Implicit attitudes as predictors of the future behavior of decided and undecided voters. Political Psychology, 29(3), 369–387. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9221.2008.00635.x
  • Arkes, H. R., & Tetlock, P. E. (2004). Attributions of implicit prejudice, or” would Jesse Jackson’fail’the Implicit Association Test?. Psychological Inquiry, 15(4), 257–278. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327965pli1504_01
  • Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L. (2000). The mind in the middle: A practical guide to priming and automaticity research. In H. T. Reis & C. M. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in social and personality psychology (pp. 253–285). Cambridge University Press.
  • Bengtson, V. L., Silverstein, M., Putney, N. M., & Harris, S. C. (2015). Does religiousness increase with age? Age changes and generational differences over 35 years. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 54(2), 363–379. https://doi.org/10.1111/jssr.12183
  • Buddhists. (2015). Buddhists. Pew Research Center. Retrieved April 10, 2017, from http://www.pewforum.org/2015/04/02/buddhists/
  • Casasanto, D. (2009). Embodiment of abstract concepts: Good and bad in right-and left-handers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 138(3), 351–367. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015854
  • Chasteen, A. L., Burdzy, D. C., & Pratt, J. (2010). Thinking of God moves attention. Neuropsychologia, 48(2), 627–630. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.09.029
  • Chelhod, J. (1973). A contribution to the problem of the pre-eminence of the right, based upon Arabic evidence. (Originally published 1964). In R. Needham (Ed.), Right & left: Essays on dual symbolic classification (pp. 239–264). University of Chicago Press.
  • Cui, L., Bu, W., Gao, Q., Wu, Q., Huang, Y., Han, X., & Luo, J. (2022). The development of discriminatory perception of junior high school students and influence on the cooperative behavior of internal and external groups. Journal of Psychology, 54(3), 259–269. https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2022.00259
  • Deaton, A. S. (2009). Aging, religion, and health (No. w15271). National Bureau of Economic Research.
  • Der, G., & Deary, I. J. (2006). Age and sex differences in reaction time in adulthood: Results from the United Kingdom Health and lifestyle survey. Psychology and Aging, 21(1), 62. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.21.1.62
  • Devos, T., & Banaji, M. R. (2005). American= white? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88(3), 447. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.88.3.447
  • Edwards, A. L. (1957). The social desirability variable in personality assessment and research. Dryden.
  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A. G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 1149–1160. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149
  • Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (2003). Implicit measures in social cognition research: Their meaning and use. Annual Review of Psychology, 54(1), 297–327. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145225
  • Fetterman, A. K. (2016). On god-belief and feeling clean: Daily experiences are related to feeling clean, particularly for those high in Godbelief. Social Psychological & Personality Science, 7, 552–559. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1948550616641474
  • Fetterman, A. K., Wilkowski, B. M., & Robinson, M. D. (2018). On feeling warm and being warm: Daily perceptions of physical warmth fluctuate with interpersonal warmth. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 9(5), 560–567. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550617712032
  • Francis, L. J., Jones, S. H., & Wilcox, C. (2000). Religiosity and happiness: During adolescence, young adulthood, and later life. Journal of Psychology & Christianity, 19(3), 245–257.
  • Goossaert, V. (2005). The concept of religion in China and the West. Diogenes, 52(1), 13–20. https://doi.org/10.1177/0392192105050596
  • Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(6), 1464–1480. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1464
  • Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the implicit association test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(2), 197–216. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.197
  • Guan, F., Ma, H., Chen, X., Zhu, D., Xiang, Y., & Chen, J. (2018). Influence of religion on prosociality: A priming study of Buddhist concepts among nonbelievers in China. The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 28(4), 281–290. https://doi.org/10.1080/10508619.2018.1520542
  • Guan, F., Xu, Z., Zhang, B., Yan, W., & Peng, K. (2020). Bidirectional associations between Tibetan Buddhism and vertical space. Psychology of Religion and Spirituality. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1037/rel0000367
  • Jin, X., Li, J., Song, W., & Zhao, T. (2020). The impact of COVID-19 and public health emergencies on consumer purchase of scarce products in China. Frontiers in Public Health, 8, 866. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpubh.2020.617166
  • Jong, J. (2013). Implicit measures in the experimental psychology of religion. In G. Dawes & J. Maclaurin (Eds.), A New Science of Religion (pp. 65–78). Taylor and Francis. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203086131
  • Khyentse, D. J. (2008). What makes you not a Buddhist. Shambhala Publications.
  • LaBouff, J. P., Rowatt, W. C., Johnson, M. K., Thedford, M., & Tsang, J. A. (2010). Development and initial validation of an implicit measure of religiousness‐spirituality. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 49(3), 439–455. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-5906.2010.01521.x
  • Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Conceptual metaphor in everyday language. The Journal of Philosophy, 77(8), 453–486. https://doi.org/10.2307/2025464
  • Landau, M. J., Meier, B. P., & Keefer, L. A. (2010). A metaphor-enriched social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 136(6), 1045–1067. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020970
  • Li, H., & Cao, Y. (2018). Karma or immortality: Can religion influence space‐time mappings? Cognitive Science, 42(3), 1041–1056. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12579
  • Mair, J. (2018). Metacognitive variety, from Inner Mongolian Buddhism to Post-Truth. In J. Proust & M. Fortier (Eds.), Metacognitive Diversity: An Interdisciplinary Approach. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780198789710.003.0018
  • Meier, B. P., & Fetterman, A. K. (2022). Metaphors for god: God is high, bright, and human in implicit tasks. Psychology of Religion and Spirituality, 14(1), 43–50. https://doi.org/10.1037/rel0000324
  • Meier, B. P., Hauser, D. J., Robinson, M. D., Friesen, C. K., & Schjeldahl, K. (2007). What’s “up” with god? Vertical space as a representation of the divine. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(5), 699–710. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.93.5.699
  • Moeller, S. K., Robinson, M. D., & Zabelina, D. L. (2008). Personality dominance and preferential use of the vertical dimension of space: Evidence from spatial attention paradigms. Psychological Science, 19(4), 355–361. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02093.x
  • Pérez, E. O. (2010). Explicit evidence on the import of implicit attitudes: The IAT and immigration policy judgments. Political Behavior, 32(4), 517–545. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-010-9115-z
  • Persich, M. R., Steinemann, S., Fetterman, A. K., & Robinson, M. D. (2018). Drawn to the light: Predicting religiosity using “god is light” metaphor. Psychology of Religion and Spirituality. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/rel0000216
  • Robinson, M. D., & Fetterman, A. K. (2014). Toward a metaphor-enriched personality psychology. In M. J. Landau, M. D. Robinson, & B. P. Meier (Eds.), The power of metaphor: Examining its influence on social life (pp. 133–152). American Psychological Association.
  • Robinson, M. D., & Neighbors, C. (2006). Catching the mind in action: Implicit methods in personality research and assessment.
  • Shariff, A. F., Cohen, A. B., & Norenzayan, A. (2008). The devil’s advocate: Secular arguments diminish both implicit and explicit religious belief. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 8(3–4), 417–423. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853708X358245
  • Sharp, C. A., Davis, E. B., George, K., Cuthbert, A. D., Zahl, B. P., Davis, D. E., Hook, J. N., & Aten, J. D. (2021). Measures of God representations: Theoretical framework and critical review. Psychology of Religion and Spirituality, 13(3), 340–357. https://doi.org/10.1037/rel0000257
  • Trzebiatowska, M., & Bruce, S. (2012). Why are women more religious than men? Oxford University Press.
  • Weeden, J., & Kurzban, R. (2013). What predicts religiosity? A multinational analysis of reproductive and cooperative morals. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34(6), 440–445. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2013.08.006

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.