439
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Implicit Learning of Prepositions in Dutch Kindergartners with and without Developmental Language Disorder

ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, & ORCID Icon

References

  • Aljahlan, Y., & Spaulding, T. (2021). Attentional shifting in children with developmental language disorder: A meta-analysis. Journal of Communication Disorders, 91(106105), 1–14. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2021.106105
  • Apple Inc. (2022). TestFlight (Version 3.2.3) [Mobile app]. App Store. https://apps.apple.com/us/app/testflight/id899247664
  • Armon-Lotem, S. (2014). Between L2 and SLI: Inflections and prepositions in the Hebrew of bilingual children with TLD and monolingual children with SLI. Journal of Child Language, 41(1), 3–33. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000912000487
  • Arnon, I. (2019). Do current statistical learning tasks capture stable individual differences in children? An investigation of task reliability across modalities. Behavioral Research Methods, 52(1), 68–81. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-019-01205-5
  • Bates, E., MacWhinney, B., Caselli, C., Devescovi, A., Natale, F., & Venza, V. (1984). A cross-linguistic study of the development of sentence interpretation strategies. Child Development, 55(2), 341–354. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129947
  • Bever, T. (1970). The cognitive basis for linguistic structures. In J. R. Hayes (Ed.), Cognition and the development of language (pp. 279–362). Wiley & Sons.
  • Bishop, D., Snowling, M., Thompson, P., & Greenhalgh, T. (2017). Phase 2 of CATALISE: A multinational multidisciplinary Delphi consensus study of problems with language development: Terminology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58(10), 1068–1080. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12721
  • Bock, K., & Griffin, Z. (2000). The persistence of structural priming: Transient activation or implicit learning? Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 129(2), 177–192. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.129.2.177
  • Bus, A., Takacs, Z., & Kegel, C. (2015). Affordances and limitations of electronic storybooks for young children’s emergent literacy. Developmental Review, 35, 79–97. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2014.12.004
  • Chang, F., Dell, G. S., & Bock, K. (2006). Becoming syntactic. Psychological Review, 113(2), 234–272. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.113.2.234
  • Chen, H., Cohen, P., & Chen, S. (2010). How big is a big odds ratio? Interpreting the magnitudes of odds ratios in epidemiological studies. Communications in Statistics - Simulation & Computation, 39(4), 860–864. https://doi.org/10.1080/03610911003650383
  • Damhuis, C., Segers, E., & Verhoeven, L. (2013). Stimulating breadth and depth of vocabulary via repeated storybook readings or tests. School Effectiveness and School Improvement, 26(3), 382–396. https://doi.org/10.1080/09243453.2014.965181
  • Dirks, E., & Wauters, L. (2015). Enhancing emergent literacy in preschool deaf and hard-of hearing children through interactive reading. In H. Knoors & M. Marschark (Eds.), Educating deaf learners: Creating a global evidence base (pp. 415–441). Oxford University Press.
  • Ebert, K., & Kohnert, K. (2011). Sustained attention in children with primary language impairment: A meta-analysis. Journal of Speech, Language, & Hearing Research, 54(5), 1372–1384. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2011/10-0231)
  • Evans, J., & MacWhinney, B. (1999). Sentence processing strategies in children with expressive and expressive-receptive specific language impairments. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 34(2), 117–134. https://doi.org/10.1080/136828299247469
  • Finneran, D., Francis, A., & Leonard, L. (2009). Sustained attention in children with SLI. Journal of Speech, Language, & Hearing Research, 52(4), 915–929. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2009/07-0053)
  • Gabriel, A., Meulemans, T., Parisse, C., & Maillart, C. (2015). Procedural learning across modalities in French-speaking children with specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 36(3), 747–769. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716413000490
  • Garraffa, M., Coco, M., & Branigan, H. (2018). Impaired implicit learning of syntactic structure in children with developmental language disorder: Evidence from syntactic priming. Autism & Developmental Language Impairments, 3, 1–15. https://doi.org/10.1177/2396941518779939
  • Grela, B., Rashiti, L., & Soares, M. (2004). Dative prepositions in children with specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25(4), 467–480. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716404001225
  • Hill, E. (2001). Non-specific nature of specific language impairment: A review of the literature with regard to concomitant motor impairments. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 36(2), 149–171. https://doi.org/10.1080/13682820118418
  • Hsu, H., & Bishop, D. (2014). Training understanding of reversible sentences: A study comparing language-impaired children with age-matched and grammar matched controls. PeerJ, 3, 1–23. https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.656
  • Kidd, E. (2012). Implicit statistical learning is directly associated with the acquisition of syntax. Developmental Psychology, 48(1), 171–184. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025405
  • Kumle, L., Võ, M., & Draschkow, D. (2021). Estimating power in (generalized) linear mixed models: An open introduction and tutorial in R. Behavioral Research Methods, 53(6), 2528–2543. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-021-01546-0
  • Lakens, D., Scheel, A., & Isager, P. (2018). Equivalence testing for psychological research: A tutorial. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 1(2), 259–269. https://doi.org/10.1177/2515245918770963
  • Lammertink, I., Boersma, P., Wijnen, F., & Rispens, J. (2020). Statistical learning in the visuomotor domain and its relation to grammar in children with and without DLD: A meta-analysis. Language Learning and Development, 16(4), 426–450. https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2020.1820340
  • Lammertink, I., Segers, E., Scheper, A., Wauters, L., & Vissers, C. (2023). Implicit learning of prepositions in Dutch kindergartners with and without developmental language disorder. Radboud University. (dataset). Version1. https://doi.org/10.34973/kj1t-4s24
  • Law, J., Garrett, Z., & Nye, C. (2004). The efficacy of treatment for children with developmental speech and language Delay/Disorder: A meta-analysis. Journal of Speech, Language, & Hearing Research, 47(4), 924–943. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2004/069)
  • Lum, J., Conti-Ramsden, G., Morgan, A., & Ullman, M. (2014). Procedural learning deficits in specific language impairment (SLI): A meta-analysis of serial reaction time task performance. Cortex, 51(100), 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2013.10.011
  • Lum, J., & Kidd, E. (2012). An examination of the associations among multiple memory systems, past tense, and vocabulary in typically developing 5-year-old children. Journal of Speech, Language, & Hearing Research, 55(4), 989–1006. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2011/10-0137)
  • Marinis, T., & van der Lely, H. (2007). On-line processing of wh-questions in children with G-SLI and typically developing children. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 42(5), 557–582. https://doi.org/10.1080/13682820601058190
  • Montgomery, J., Evans, J., & Gillam, R. (2018). Memory and language in children with SLI. In T. Alloway (Ed.), Working memory and clinical developmental disorders (pp. 22–37). Routledge.
  • Nelson, K. (1974). Variations in children’s concepts by age and category. Child Development, 45(3), 577–584. https://doi.org/10.2307/1127823
  • Nicholas, K., Alt, M., & Hauwiller, E. (2019). Variability of input in preposition learning by preschoolers with developmental language disorder and typically-developing language. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 35(1), 55–74. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265659019830455
  • Nunnally, J., & Bernstein, I. (1994). Psychometric theory (3rd ed.). McGraw-Hill.
  • Obeid, R., Brooks, P., Powers, K., Gillespie-Lynch, K., & Lum, J. (2016). Statistical learning in specific language impairment and autism spectrum disorder: A meta- analysis. Frontiers in Psychology, 7(1245). https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01245
  • Oliveira, C., Henderson, L., & Hayiou-Thomas, M. (2022). Limited evidence of an association between language, literacy and procedural learning in typical and atypical development: A meta-analysis. Cognitive Science. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/ndtg6
  • Pauls, L., & Archibald, L. (2016). Executive functions in children with specific language impairment: A meta-analysis. Journal of Speech, Language, & Hearing Research, 59(5), 1074–1086. https://doi.org/10.1044/2016_JSLHR-L-15-0174
  • Peirce, J., (2019) PsychoPy3 (Version 3.6.6) [Computer Software]. Open Science Tools Ltd. https://www.psychopy.org
  • Puglisi, M., Befi-Lopes, D., & Takiuchi, N. (2005). Utilização e compreensão de preposições por crianças com distúrbio específico de linguagem [Use and comprehension of prepositions by children with specific language impairment]. Pró-Fono Revista de Atualização Científica, 17(3), 331–344. https://doi.org/10.1590/s0104-56872005000300007
  • Raven, J. (1984). Manual for the coloured progressive matrices: Dutch Version van bon, W. Swets & Zeitlinger B.V.
  • R Core Team. (2022). R: A language and environment for statistical computing (version 4.2.2) [Computer software]. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. https://www.r-project.org
  • Semel, E., Wiig, E., & Secord, W. (2010). Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals: Dutch version. (3rd ed , W. Kort, E. Compaan, M. Schittekatte, & P. Dekker, Eds.). Pearson. [Measurement instrument].
  • Skeide, M., & Friederici, A. (2016). The ontogeny of the cortical language network. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 17(5), 323–332. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2016.23
  • Smeets, D., van Dijken, M., & Bus, A. (2014). Using electronic storybooks to support word learning in children with severe language impairments. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 47(5), 435–449. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219412467069
  • Streiner, D. (2003). Starting at the beginning: An introduction to coefficient alpha and internal consistency. Journal of Personality Assessment, 80(1), 99–103. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327752JPA8001_18
  • Takacs, Z., Swart, E., & Bus, A. (2015). Benefits and pitfalls of multimedia and interactive features in technology-enhanced storybooks: A meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 85(4), 698–739. https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654314566989
  • Tomas, E., & Vissers, C. (2019). Behind the scenes of developmental language disorder: Time to call neuropsychology back on stage. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 12(517). https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2018.00517
  • Ullman, M., & Pierpont, E. (2005). Specific language impairment is not specific to language: The procedural deficit hypothesis. Cortex, 41(3), 399–433. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70276-4
  • Unity Technologies. (2019). Unity real-time development platform. (version 2019.4.40f1) [Computer Software] https://store.unity.com
  • van der Hoek-Snieders, H., Lijten, I., Van Klaveren, E., Jansen-Spit, S., & Bruinsma, G. (2021). Voorzetselgebruik van kinderen met TOS: Een vergelijking met zich normaal ontwikkelende kinderen [Preposition use in children with DLD: A comparison with typically developing children]. Nederlands tijdschrift voor Logopedie, 93(2), 19–26.
  • Van der Lely, H., & Dewart, H.(1986). Sentence comprehension strategies in specifically language impaired children. The British Journal of Disorders of Communication, 21(3), 291–306. https://doi.org/10.3109/13682828609019843
  • van der Lely, H., & Harris, M. (1990). Comprehension of reversible sentences in specifically language-impaired children. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Disorders, 55(1), 101–117. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshd.5501.101
  • Vasilyeva, M., Huttenlocher, J., & Waterfall, H. (2006). Effects of language intervention on syntactic skill levels in preschoolers. Developmental Psychology, 41(1), 164–174. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.42.1.164
  • Vissers, C., Koolen, S., Hermans, D., Scheper, A., & Knoors, H. (2015). Executive functioning in preschoolers with specific language impairment. Frontiers Psychology, 6(1574). https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01574
  • Von Berger, E., Wulfeck, B., Bates, E., & Fink, N. (1996). Developmental changes in real-time sentence processing. First Language, 16(47), 193–222. https://doi.org/10.1177/014272379601604703
  • Vugs, B., Cuperus, J., Hendriks, M., & Verhoeven, L. (2013). Visuospatial working memory in specific language impairment: A meta-analysis. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34(9), 2586–2597. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2013.05.014
  • Watkins, R., & Rice, M. (1991). Verb particle and preposition acquisition in language-impaired preschoolers. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 34(5), 1130–1141. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3405.1130
  • West, G., Vadillo, M., Shanks, D., & Hulme, C. (2017). The procedural learning deficit hypothesis of language learning disorders: We see some problems. Developmental Science, 21(2), 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12552
  • Zwart, F., Vissers, C., Kessels, R., & Maes, J. (2018). Implicit learning seems to come naturally for children with autism, but not for children with specific language impairment: Evidence from behavioral and ERP data. Autism Research, 11(7), 1050–1061. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1954
  • Zwart, F., Vissers, C., & Maes, J. (2018). The association between sequence learning on the serial reaction time task and social impairments in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disabilities, 48(8), 2692–2700. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-018-3529-6